Loading

Annals of Pediatrics and Child Health

Remediating Developmental Dyslexia: Two Hands Are Better Than One

Review Article | Open Access | Volume 10 | Issue 5

  • 1. Department of Curriculum and Instruction, University of Victoria, Canada
+ Show More - Show Less
Corresponding Authors
Department of Curriculum and Instruction, University of Victoria, 2368 Rozon Rd., Mill. Bay, BC V0R2P4, Canada
ABSTRACT

This article describes a unique multisensory approach to dyslexic remediation, suitable for children beyond the age of 7 years, that involves the use of both hands; the left for learning the spatial order and the right for learning the sequential order of letters in syllables.

KEYWORDS

• Dyslexia; Remediation; Learning; Syllables

CITATION

Mather DS. Remediating Developmental Dyslexia: Two Hands Are Better Than One. Ann Pediatr Child Health 2022; 10(5): 1282.

INTRODUCTION

Over 90 years ago, in an analysis of the lateral dominance of approximately 100 cases of extreme difficulty in learning to read, Walter Dearborn [1], found a “preponderance in the clinical cases of (1) left-eyedness, of (2) the lack of ocular and manual dominance, and (3) of mixed conditions of ocular and manual dominance, e.g., left-eyedness associated with right handedness or ambidexterity”. He noted that “The dextral sequence of eye movements is kinesthetically the essence of reading” and that “Left-eyed children may tend to move in the opposite direction, to begin at the wrong end of words or even to perceive letters in the wrong way as in seeing b as d, or boy as dog” (p. 704). In accord, a recent review concluded that dyslexia develops in left eye-right brain dominant children when compensating for eye tracking that is reversed to the left-to-right order of letters in words [2]. Consequently, “the fundamental deficit in developmental dyslexia may be one in the spatiotemporal attentional sampling of the visual input which can lead not only to poor reading ability but potentially also to poorer phonological skills” [3].

A procedure which appears to address this spatiotemporal attentional sampling deficit was developed by a Junior High School remedial teacher [4]: Zorotovich wrote words with syllable breaks in large cursive script on slips of paper. Students were then taught to first pronounce the syllables while tracing them with the non-writing forefinger and immediately following to simultaneously ‘pound’ and name their letter sequence with the writing hand forefinger (both actions countering any hand/ eye tendency to move in a right-to-left direction). She claimed that this method developed sight word and phonological coding in reading-disabled students in a remarkably short time. Her claim is bolstered by evidence of non-preferred hand superiority in tactile-kinesthetic word-learning [5]. As well, rightward letter-name ‘pounding’ may rectify the dissociation between procedural learning of letter names and hand-motor sequencing found in university and college dyslexic readers by Gabay, Schiff & Vakil [6]. Zorotovich’s method is also congruent with remedial success achieved by multisensory teaching (Johnston, 2019) and by ameliorating global-local [7], and ground-figure [8], precedence deficits in dyslexia. These results argue against the assumption that reading deficiencies in dyslexia are caused by phonological or language deficits and thus require a paradigm shift from a phonologically-based treatment to a visually-based treatment of dyslexia. More generally it resonates with a “framecontent” (global-local/ground-figure) view of language evolution [9], and therefore may be universally applicable to the teaching of reading.

Guiard [9], in the context of explaining bimanual action, argued that there is “a general hierarchy principle according to which human action, whether perceptual, intellectual, or motor, preferentially proceeds from the macro- to the micro-metric, that is, outlines global traits before delineating more detailed aspects” (p. 502) and that “[t]o use the terms of MacNeilage, Studdert-Kennedy and Lindblom [10], we may say that the left hand delineates ‘frames’ into which the activity of the right hand inserts ‘contents’” (p. 494). MacNeilage et al. [10], compared the activity of the left and right hands to that of two contiguous motors in a kinematic chain, noting that with a single motor “one must be either farsighted or near-sighted in action” (p. 513).

Using Zorotovich’s procedure, I found that ten 1/2 hour sessions with two Junior High School 12-year-old reading disabled boys produced twice the improvement in reading rate and accuracy compared to that obtained by Shaywitz, B., Shaywitz, S., Blachman, B. et al. [11], with a 105 hours per child, one-on-one phonics intervention with reading disabled children aged 6.1 to 9.4 years. With the two Junior High School 12-year-old boys, I found that any reasonable word-syllable pronunciation subdivision was acceptable as long as each syllable contained a vowel. For example: trans port a tion, tran spor ta tion, or tran spor ta tion. Counter-intuitively, practice with multi-syllable words leads to more word pattern (orthographic) learning than single syllable words: It also speeds syllable boundary sensitivity because more frequent letter patterns occur within rather than across syllable boundaries. Moreover, multi-syllable words have been found to motivate intermediate grade learners because they felt they were no longer dealing with ‘baby’ words [12]. A syllable-based learning strategy for the mastery of middle school science vocabulary is available [13], as well as a frequency list of the most commonly occurring multisyllabic science words in texts encountered by adolescents [14]. The following task may be used to assess the effects of this intervention:

ANIMAL ORDERING

Students place animal tokens on a horizontal series of empty circles, ordered so that their favorites are first in the line to get fed. The instructions leave out whether the head of the line begins on the left or the right side. A University of British Columbia study administered this task to 547 to public school children in grades K,1,2,4 and 6 [15]. It found a high percentage of right-to left organizers [K(35), 1(37), 2(29), 4(17), 6(16)], indicating that it may be sensitive to the left-to-right directional problem common to both reading and spelling/handwriting difficulties.

The specifics of the technique are as follows:

1. Aided by the instructor, the student claps and pronounces the syllable chunks of a selected word.

2. The instructor then writes the whole word in large cursive script, underlining the syllables (Zorotovich used 2 inch by15 inch word slips).

3. With the non-writing forefinger, each syllable is then simultaneously traced and pronounced by the student, followed by “air-writing” with the writing hand before pounding. This provides an arm-to-finger (whole-to-part) progression that is consistent with handwriting learning instruction and may eliminate repeated pounding in order to recall spelling sequences. Immediately following, each syllable letter sequence is simultaneously pounded and named with the forefinger of the writing hand. This cycle is repeated until the word can be cursively written correctly from memory. Importantly, the student pronounces each syllable either to himself or aloud as he traces it and as he writes it. Pronunciation is “stretched” so as to end with the last letter traced. Cursive writing by the student is essential for complete success with this intervention as it has been found to be an advantage for children with dyslexia and handwriting difficulties compared to those without these difficulties [16]. In teaching cursive writing it is essential that the teaching process emphasize the orderly and rhythmical flow of those movements which cursive writing naturally encourages. If the teacher does not exploit this built-in aspect of cursive writing, then cursive writing alone probably will not help. It is most important that in early cursive writing the student uses large letters, six to eight inches high. As the child gains competence with large letters, the size can be reduced progressively. The child first learning to control fairly gross movements of his arm in writing and progressively bringing the fine movements of his hand and fingers into play. This gross-to-fine sequence is developmentally sound in view of the proximo-distal developmental sequence.

“Too often, children are required to use fine movements before gross movements are efficient” [17]. As well, using a white board and a dry/erase marker may facilitate handwriting rhythm.

This procedure may only be successful with dyslexic children beyond 7 years-of-age. The age of 8 years is a developmental milestone for the integration of vision with proprioception [a term interchangeable with kinesthesia [18]. For example, eight-yearold but not six-year-old children are able to map proprioceptive space onto visual space in a line-drawing visuomotor adaptation task [19]. And in a mirror-drawing task in which 5-to-6 year old and 7-to-8 year old children practiced tracing a square while looking at the reflection of their hand in a mirror, only the older group was able to re-calibrate their perceptual and sensorimotor systems to learn the required visual-proprioceptive mapping [20]. This proprioceptive/kinesthetic developmental faculty may be essential for recalling the spatial order of letters in syllables while tracing with the non-writing forefinger and the sequential order of letters while pounding with the right forefinger. Congruently, several studies have found that the development of bimanual coordination is not attained until 8 years-of-age [21- 26]. Thus, de Boer et al. [21], found that in-phase (mirror) and anti-phase (non mirror) coordination developed in parallel from age 7 with no evidence of enhanced inhibition of mirror activity. In turn, Fagard & Pezé [23], concluded that the disappearance of mirror and parallel performance differences between the two hands at 8 years-of-age implied “[a] relationship between lateralized control of the two hands as a coordinative structure and the maturation of interhemispheric communication . . .” (p. 82). Correspondingly, at 8 years-of-age bimanual temporal sequencing develops in the dominant hand leading condition [25], and the non-dominant hand achieves a similar level of performance as the leading dominant hand [26], consistent with RH proprioceptive movement dominance for both arms [27,28]. Denckla [22], speculated that the motor-skill developmental pattern of right-sided function before a rapid increase in leftsided function at 8 years-of-age could be due to the maturation of interhemisphere connections. In line with this speculation, the preferred and non-preferred arms respectively rely on LH visual guidance and RH proprioceptive feedback [27], faculties that do not integrate before 8 years-of-age. Furthermore, between the ages of 5 and 8 there is dramatically improved proprioceptive utilization with only minor improvement during adolescence and adulthood [29-31]. In sum, it appears that by the time children reach their eighth birthday “from the cooperative action of two arms there emerges, so to speak, a superordinate arm” (Guiard, 1987, p. 512) capable of interhemispheric coordination of left hand mirror and right hand non mirror movements.

Mesker (a child neurologist) had a different stance [32]. From extensive experience with Dutch children affected by learning disorders, he found that many were slow to achieve lateralization of hand dominance, remaining in a bilateral proximal stage of development until 8 years-of-age. He claimed that writing before this age could result in manual-motor and ocular-motor habits detrimental to the development of reading and spelling proficiency. Congruently, there is evidence in dyslexia of impaired bimanual [34], and binocular coordination [34-36]. Regrettably, Mesker’s clinical results lacked empirical validation and have received attention only in Holland [37].

DISCUSSION

The present article describes a unique multisensory approach for remediating children with dyslexia who are above the age of 7 years-of-age It involves the use of both hands, the left hand in learning the global order and the right hand learning the sequential order of letters in syllables. The method is consistent with a global-to-local/ground-figure view of language evolution and therefore may be universally applicable to the teaching of reading. It may remediate both major subtypes of dyslexia: the one in which poor phonological decoding is associated with good reading comprehension and the other in which good decoding proficiency is associated with poor reading comprehension [38]. In the latter, poor reading comprehension is manifested by difficulty in globally decoding words that are typically read with semantic support [39,40]. Conversely, good comprehension dyslexia relies heavily on semantic support in reading [39]. This multisensory bimanual procedure, discovered by Zorotovich [4], argues against the assumption that reading deficiencies in dyslexia are caused by phonological or language deficits and thus requires a paradigm shift from phonologically-based treatments to visually-based treatments of dyslexia [41].

REFERENCES
  1. Dearborn WF. Ocular and manual dominance in dyslexia. Psychological Bulletin. 1931; 28: 704.
  2. Mather DS, Milford TM, McRae LM. Does dyslexia develop from left eye dominance? Perceptual and Motor Skills. 2015; 121: 569-601.
  3. Vidyasagar TR. Aetiology of dyslexia: a visual perspective on a phonological marker. In J. Stein, Z. Kapoula (Eds.), Visual Aspects of Dyslexia: Oxford University Press. 2012; 151-170.
  4. Zorotovich B. Three strange relatives. Academic Therapy Quarterly. 1985; 21: 223-227.
  5. Traynelis-Yurek E. Preferred versus Non-Preferred Hand. Academic Therapy Quarterly. 1985; 21: 29-36.
  6. Gabay Y, Schiff R, Vakil E. Dissociation between the procedural learning of letter names and motor sequences in developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia. 2012; 50: 2435-2441.
  7. Franceschini S, Bertoni S, Gianesini T, Gori S, Facoetti A. A different vision of dyslexia: Local precedence on global perception. Scientific Reports. 2017; 7: 10.
  8. Lawton T, Shelley-Tremblay J. Training on movement figure-ground discrimination remediates low-level visual timing deficits in the dorsal stream, improving high-level cognitive functioning, including attention, reading fluency, and working memory. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2017: 11.
  9. Guiard Y. Asymmetric division of labor in human skilled bimanual action. J Motor Behavior. 1980; 19: 486-517.
  10. MacNeilage P, Studdert-Kennedy M, Lindblom B. Functional precursors to language and its lateralization. Am J Physiol. 1980; 246: R912-R914.
  11. Shaywitz B, Shaywitz S, Blachman B, Pugh KR, Fulbright RK, Skudlarski P, et al. Development of left occipitotemporal systems for skilled reading in children after a phonologically- based intervention. Biological Psychiatry. 2004; 55: 926-933
  12. Shefelbine J. A syllabic-unit approach to teaching decoding of polysyllabic words to fourth- and sixth-grade disabled readers. In J. Zutell & S. McCormick (Eds.), Literacy theory and research: Analysis from multiple paradigms (pp. 223–230). Chicago: National Reading Conference. 1990.
  13. Bhattacharya A. Syllable-based reading strategy for mastery of scientific information. Remedial and Special Education. 2006; 27: 116-123.
  14. Nellenbach K, Zoski J, Diamond J, Erickson K. A graphomorphemic approach to identifying and selecting a set of high utility, stable affixes common to the technical vocabulary of science. Applied Psycholinguistics. 2014; 36: 1441-1457.
  15. Johnson G. Directional biases in children’s perception of spatial order. J Experimental Education. 1990; 59: 19-29.
  16. Arfé B, Corato F, Pizzocaro E, Merella S. The effects of script and orthographic complexity on the handwriting and spelling performance of children with dyslexia. J Learning Disabilities. 2020; 53: 96-108.
  17. Early G. The case for cursive writing. Academic Therapy. 1973; 9: 105- 108.
  18. Stillman BC. Making sense of proprioception. The meaning of proprioception, kinaesthesia and related terms. Physiotherapy. 2022; 88: 667-676.
  19. Contreras-Vidal J, Bo J, Boudreau J, Clark J. Development of visuomotor representations for hand movement in young children. Experimental Brain Res. 2004; 162: 155-164.
  20. Julius M, Adi-Japha E. A developmental perspective in learning the mirror-drawing task. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2016; 10: 1-13.
  21. de Boer B, Peper C, Beek P. Development of temporal and spatial bimanual coordination during childhood. Motor Control. 2012; 16: 537-559.
  22. Denckla M. Development of motor co-ordination in normal children. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology. 1974; 16: 729-741.
  23. Fagard J, Pezé A. Coupling and lateralization in bimanual coordination at 7, 8, and 9 years of age. Developmental Neuropsychology. 1992; 8: 69-85.
  24. Riquelme I, Arnould C, Hatem S, Bleyenheuft Y. The two-arm coordination test: maturation of bimanual coordination in typically developing children and deficits in children with unilateral cerebral palsy. Developmental Neurorehabilitation. 2018; 22: 312-320.
  25. Rudisch J, Butler J, Izadi H, Birtles D, Green D. Developmental characteristics of disparate bimanual movement skills in typically developing children. J Motor Behavior. 2017; 50: 8-16.
  26. Serrien D, Sovijärvi-Spapé M, Rana G. Developmental changes in motor control: Insights from bimanual coordination. Developmental Psychology. 2014; 50: 316-323.
  27. Goble D, Brown S. Upper limb asymmetries in the matching of proprioceptive versus visual targets. J Neurophysiology. 2018; 99: 3063-3074.
  28. Verschueren S, Swinnen S, Cordo P, Dounskaia N. Proprioceptive control of multijoint movement: bimanual circle drawing. Experimental Brain Res. 1999; 127: 182-192.
  29. Hay L, Redon C. The control of goal-directed movements in children: Role of proprioceptive muscle afferents. Human Movement Science. 1997; 16: 433-451.
  30. Sigmundsson H, Whiting HTA, Loftesnes JM. Development of proprioceptive sensitivity. Experimental Brain Res. 2000; 135: 348- 352.
  31. von Hofsten C, Rosblad B. The integration of sensory information in the development of precise manual pointing. Neuropsychologia. 1988; 26: 805–821.
  32. Mesker P. (1969/1980). [The human hand: a study into the development of skill in relation to the cerebral organization in learning disordered children]. Nijmegen, The Netherlands: Dekker and Van de Vegt. [in Dutch]
  33. Gladstone M, Best CT, Davidson R.J. Anomalous bimanual coordination among dyslexic boys. Developmental Psychology. 1989; 25: 236-246.
  34. Bucci M, Kapoula Z. Binocular coordination of saccades in 7 years old children in single word reading and target fixation. Vision Res. 2006; 46: 457-466.
  35. Jainta S, Kapoula Z. Dyslexic children are confronted with unstable binocular fixation while reading. PLOS ONE. 2011; 6: e18694.
  36. Kirkby JA, Blythe HI, Drieghe D, Liversedge SP. Reading text increases binocular disparity in dyslexic children. PLOS ONE. 2011; 6: e27105.
  37. van Grunsven W, Vuylsteke-Wauters M, Njiokiktjien C, Vranken M. Ontogenesis of laterality in 3- to 10-yr.-old children: Increased unimanual independence grounded on improved bimanual motor function. Perceptual And Motor Skills. 2009; 109: 3-29.
  38. Catts HW, Adlof SM, Ellis Weismer S. Language deficits in poor comprehenders: a case for the simple view of reading. J Speech, Language, and Hearing Res. 2006; 49: 278-293.
  39. Nation K, Snowling MJ. Semantic processing and the development of word recognition skills: Evidence from children with reading comprehension difficulties. J Memory and Language. 1998; 39: 85- 101.
  40. Ricketts J, Bishop DVM, Nation K. Investigating orthographic and semantic aspects of word learning in poor comprehenders. J Res Reading. 2008; 31: 117-135.
  41. Denckla MB. Childhood learning disabilities. In K.M. Heilman, & E. Valenstein (Eds.), Clinical neuropsychology. New York: Oxford University Press. 1979.

Mather DS. Remediating Developmental Dyslexia: Two Hands Are Better Than One. Ann Pediatr Child Health 2022; 10(5): 1282.

Received : 11 Sep 2022
Accepted : 12 Oct 2022
Published : 15 Oct 2022
Journals
Annals of Otolaryngology and Rhinology
ISSN : 2379-948X
Launched : 2014
JSM Schizophrenia
Launched : 2016
Journal of Nausea
Launched : 2020
JSM Internal Medicine
Launched : 2016
JSM Hepatitis
Launched : 2016
JSM Oro Facial Surgeries
ISSN : 2578-3211
Launched : 2016
Journal of Human Nutrition and Food Science
ISSN : 2333-6706
Launched : 2013
JSM Regenerative Medicine and Bioengineering
ISSN : 2379-0490
Launched : 2013
JSM Spine
ISSN : 2578-3181
Launched : 2016
Archives of Palliative Care
ISSN : 2573-1165
Launched : 2016
JSM Nutritional Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3203
Launched : 2017
Annals of Neurodegenerative Disorders
ISSN : 2476-2032
Launched : 2016
Journal of Fever
ISSN : 2641-7782
Launched : 2017
JSM Bone Marrow Research
ISSN : 2578-3351
Launched : 2016
JSM Mathematics and Statistics
ISSN : 2578-3173
Launched : 2014
Journal of Autoimmunity and Research
ISSN : 2573-1173
Launched : 2014
JSM Arthritis
ISSN : 2475-9155
Launched : 2016
JSM Head and Neck Cancer-Cases and Reviews
ISSN : 2573-1610
Launched : 2016
JSM General Surgery Cases and Images
ISSN : 2573-1564
Launched : 2016
JSM Anatomy and Physiology
ISSN : 2573-1262
Launched : 2016
JSM Dental Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1548
Launched : 2016
Annals of Emergency Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1017
Launched : 2016
Annals of Mens Health and Wellness
ISSN : 2641-7707
Launched : 2017
Journal of Preventive Medicine and Health Care
ISSN : 2576-0084
Launched : 2018
Journal of Chronic Diseases and Management
ISSN : 2573-1300
Launched : 2016
Annals of Vaccines and Immunization
ISSN : 2378-9379
Launched : 2014
JSM Heart Surgery Cases and Images
ISSN : 2578-3157
Launched : 2016
Annals of Reproductive Medicine and Treatment
ISSN : 2573-1092
Launched : 2016
JSM Brain Science
ISSN : 2573-1289
Launched : 2016
JSM Biomarkers
ISSN : 2578-3815
Launched : 2014
JSM Biology
ISSN : 2475-9392
Launched : 2016
Archives of Stem Cell and Research
ISSN : 2578-3580
Launched : 2014
Annals of Clinical and Medical Microbiology
ISSN : 2578-3629
Launched : 2014
JSM Pediatric Surgery
ISSN : 2578-3149
Launched : 2017
Journal of Memory Disorder and Rehabilitation
ISSN : 2578-319X
Launched : 2016
JSM Tropical Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2578-3165
Launched : 2016
JSM Head and Face Medicine
ISSN : 2578-3793
Launched : 2016
JSM Cardiothoracic Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1297
Launched : 2016
JSM Bone and Joint Diseases
ISSN : 2578-3351
Launched : 2017
JSM Bioavailability and Bioequivalence
ISSN : 2641-7812
Launched : 2017
JSM Atherosclerosis
ISSN : 2573-1270
Launched : 2016
Journal of Genitourinary Disorders
ISSN : 2641-7790
Launched : 2017
Journal of Fractures and Sprains
ISSN : 2578-3831
Launched : 2016
Journal of Autism and Epilepsy
ISSN : 2641-7774
Launched : 2016
Annals of Marine Biology and Research
ISSN : 2573-105X
Launched : 2014
JSM Health Education & Primary Health Care
ISSN : 2578-3777
Launched : 2016
JSM Communication Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3807
Launched : 2016
Annals of Musculoskeletal Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3599
Launched : 2016
Annals of Virology and Research
ISSN : 2573-1122
Launched : 2014
JSM Renal Medicine
ISSN : 2573-1637
Launched : 2016
Journal of Muscle Health
ISSN : 2578-3823
Launched : 2016
JSM Genetics and Genomics
ISSN : 2334-1823
Launched : 2013
JSM Anxiety and Depression
ISSN : 2475-9139
Launched : 2016
Clinical Journal of Heart Diseases
ISSN : 2641-7766
Launched : 2016
Annals of Medicinal Chemistry and Research
ISSN : 2378-9336
Launched : 2014
JSM Pain and Management
ISSN : 2578-3378
Launched : 2016
JSM Women's Health
ISSN : 2578-3696
Launched : 2016
Clinical Research in HIV or AIDS
ISSN : 2374-0094
Launched : 2013
Journal of Endocrinology, Diabetes and Obesity
ISSN : 2333-6692
Launched : 2013
Journal of Substance Abuse and Alcoholism
ISSN : 2373-9363
Launched : 2013
JSM Neurosurgery and Spine
ISSN : 2373-9479
Launched : 2013
Journal of Liver and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2379-0830
Launched : 2014
Journal of Drug Design and Research
ISSN : 2379-089X
Launched : 2014
JSM Clinical Oncology and Research
ISSN : 2373-938X
Launched : 2013
JSM Bioinformatics, Genomics and Proteomics
ISSN : 2576-1102
Launched : 2014
JSM Chemistry
ISSN : 2334-1831
Launched : 2013
Journal of Trauma and Care
ISSN : 2573-1246
Launched : 2014
JSM Surgical Oncology and Research
ISSN : 2578-3688
Launched : 2016
Annals of Food Processing and Preservation
ISSN : 2573-1033
Launched : 2016
Journal of Radiology and Radiation Therapy
ISSN : 2333-7095
Launched : 2013
JSM Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation
ISSN : 2578-3572
Launched : 2016
Annals of Clinical Pathology
ISSN : 2373-9282
Launched : 2013
Annals of Cardiovascular Diseases
ISSN : 2641-7731
Launched : 2016
Journal of Behavior
ISSN : 2576-0076
Launched : 2016
Annals of Clinical and Experimental Metabolism
ISSN : 2572-2492
Launched : 2016
Clinical Research in Infectious Diseases
ISSN : 2379-0636
Launched : 2013
JSM Microbiology
ISSN : 2333-6455
Launched : 2013
Journal of Urology and Research
ISSN : 2379-951X
Launched : 2014
Journal of Family Medicine and Community Health
ISSN : 2379-0547
Launched : 2013
Annals of Pregnancy and Care
ISSN : 2578-336X
Launched : 2017
JSM Cell and Developmental Biology
ISSN : 2379-061X
Launched : 2013
Annals of Aquaculture and Research
ISSN : 2379-0881
Launched : 2014
Clinical Research in Pulmonology
ISSN : 2333-6625
Launched : 2013
Journal of Immunology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2333-6714
Launched : 2013
Annals of Forensic Research and Analysis
ISSN : 2378-9476
Launched : 2014
JSM Biochemistry and Molecular Biology
ISSN : 2333-7109
Launched : 2013
Annals of Breast Cancer Research
ISSN : 2641-7685
Launched : 2016
Annals of Gerontology and Geriatric Research
ISSN : 2378-9409
Launched : 2014
Journal of Sleep Medicine and Disorders
ISSN : 2379-0822
Launched : 2014
JSM Burns and Trauma
ISSN : 2475-9406
Launched : 2016
Chemical Engineering and Process Techniques
ISSN : 2333-6633
Launched : 2013
Annals of Clinical Cytology and Pathology
ISSN : 2475-9430
Launched : 2014
JSM Allergy and Asthma
ISSN : 2573-1254
Launched : 2016
Journal of Neurological Disorders and Stroke
ISSN : 2334-2307
Launched : 2013
Annals of Sports Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2379-0571
Launched : 2014
JSM Sexual Medicine
ISSN : 2578-3718
Launched : 2016
Annals of Vascular Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2378-9344
Launched : 2014
JSM Biotechnology and Biomedical Engineering
ISSN : 2333-7117
Launched : 2013
Journal of Hematology and Transfusion
ISSN : 2333-6684
Launched : 2013
JSM Environmental Science and Ecology
ISSN : 2333-7141
Launched : 2013
Journal of Cardiology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2333-6676
Launched : 2013
JSM Nanotechnology and Nanomedicine
ISSN : 2334-1815
Launched : 2013
Journal of Ear, Nose and Throat Disorders
ISSN : 2475-9473
Launched : 2016
JSM Ophthalmology
ISSN : 2333-6447
Launched : 2013
Journal of Pharmacology and Clinical Toxicology
ISSN : 2333-7079
Launched : 2013
Annals of Psychiatry and Mental Health
ISSN : 2374-0124
Launched : 2013
Medical Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology
ISSN : 2333-6439
Launched : 2013
JSM Clinical Pharmaceutics
ISSN : 2379-9498
Launched : 2014
JSM Foot and Ankle
ISSN : 2475-9112
Launched : 2016
JSM Alzheimer's Disease and Related Dementia
ISSN : 2378-9565
Launched : 2014
Journal of Addiction Medicine and Therapy
ISSN : 2333-665X
Launched : 2013
Journal of Veterinary Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2378-931X
Launched : 2013
Annals of Public Health and Research
ISSN : 2378-9328
Launched : 2014
Annals of Orthopedics and Rheumatology
ISSN : 2373-9290
Launched : 2013
Journal of Clinical Nephrology and Research
ISSN : 2379-0652
Launched : 2014
Annals of Community Medicine and Practice
ISSN : 2475-9465
Launched : 2014
Annals of Biometrics and Biostatistics
ISSN : 2374-0116
Launched : 2013
JSM Clinical Case Reports
ISSN : 2373-9819
Launched : 2013
Journal of Cancer Biology and Research
ISSN : 2373-9436
Launched : 2013
Journal of Surgery and Transplantation Science
ISSN : 2379-0911
Launched : 2013
Journal of Dermatology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2373-9371
Launched : 2013
JSM Gastroenterology and Hepatology
ISSN : 2373-9487
Launched : 2013
Annals of Nursing and Practice
ISSN : 2379-9501
Launched : 2014
JSM Dentistry
ISSN : 2333-7133
Launched : 2013
Author Information X