Loading

Annals of Forensic Research and Analysis

Criminal Prosecutors: Highly Assessed Lie-Detection Abilities and Beliefs about Defendants’ Deception

Research Article | Open Access

  • 1. Department of Behavioral Sciences, Ariel University, Israel
+ Show More - Show Less
Corresponding Authors
Eitan Elaad, Department of Behavioral Sciences, Ariel University, Ariel 40700, Israel
Abstract

Criminal prosecutors and laypersons were asked to self-assess their abilities to tell and detect lies tell truths and believe others. Prosecutors highly assessed their abilities to detect lies in others and rated them significantly higher than laypersons. They assessed their lie-detection ability significantly higher than their believing ability. Prosecutor’s beliefs about various aspects of defendants’ deception in the court of law were also discussed.

Keywords


•    Criminal
•    Prosecutors
•    Deception
•    Defendant

Citation

Elaad E (2015) Criminal Prosecutors: Highly Assessed Lie-Detection Abilities and Beliefs about Defendants’ Deception. Ann Forensic Res Anal 2(2): 1017.

INTRODUCTION

Accuracy in deception detection experiments has been found to be only slightly above chance level and almost never exceeded 60% where chance expectancy is 50% [1].Furthermore, even professionals who are regularly engaged in detecting deceptions, such as federal law enforcement officers were unable to distinguish between truthful and deceptive messages [2]. Vrij and Semin [3] attributed this to a lack of on the job feedback available to professional lie-detectors about whether their judgments were accurate or inaccurate.

In this context it is at odds that people tend to overestimate their lie-detection ability [4-7]. The lie-detection bias was explained by the prevailing norms that lead people not allow themselves to be easily deceived by others. Therefore, people would like to believe they possess the ability to detect lies.

People tend to overestimate their own ability to tell the truth convincingly [6-8]. The bias was explained by the “illusion of transparency” [9] which suggests that in communications, senders are anchored to their own internal experience. Although they realize that recipients are not exposed to the same information as they are their adjustment is insufficient [10] and they tend to believe that the receiver has the ability to discern their internal states and find out that they are telling the truth. Furthermore, people wish to sustain a positive self-image [11]. In this context, the ability to convince when telling the truth is a desired disposition.

The perceived ability to believe others is usually assessed slightly but not significantly above average [6-8]. Finally, people are inclined to underestimate their lie-telling ability [4-8,12]. The underestimated lie-telling ability was also explained by the illusion of transparency [9] and by the wish to sustain a positive self-image. Thus, if I am not an able lie-teller I am an honest person. In the present study, criminal prosecutors and people from the community were asked to self-assess their abilities to tell and detect lies, tell truths and believe others compared to other people. Based on previous research on police interrogators [5- 6], it was hypothesized that criminal prosecutors will perceive themselves as being better lie-detectors than lay-people. In addition, an attempt was made to investigate beliefs of prosecutors about defendants’ deception.

Earlier accounts reported that lie experts had the same incorrect beliefs about deceptive behavior as inexperienced laypersons [3,6,13]. That police interrogators believe that they do not interrogate innocent suspects [14]. That special training in deception detection leads police investigators to make prejudgments of deception [14]. That police investigators put more pressure on the suspect to facilitate confessions when they assume deception [15]. These earlier reports were tested again with a group of prosecutors.

METHODS

Participants

Participants were 32 Israeli criminal prosecutors (14 males, 18 females) with a mean age of 35.6 (SD=7.3) (range = 28-60) years. Their average experience as attorneys was 8.9 (SD= 8.6) (range = 2-34) years. Their average experience as prosecutors was 7.2 (SD=6.1) (range= 2-21) years. A control group of 32 persons from the community (14 males, 18 females) was also recruited. Their mean age was 33.78 (SD=12.4) (range 23 –59). All participants were volunteers who gave their written consent to participate in the study after receiving assurance of confidentiality and anonymity.

Materials and procedure Participants were first asked to self-assess their own liedetection, lie-telling, truth-telling and believing abilities in comparison with others. For example, participants were asked: “Compared to other people, how would you assess your own ability to detect lies in others?”Answers ranged from 0 (much worse than other people) to 100 (much better than other people) with 50 (as good as others) serving as the middle point. They completed the questionnaire individually with no time constrain. Prosecutors were further presented with 26 statements about different aspects of practice in the court of law. For example, they were asked to indicate their agreement with the statement: “While cross-examining I am highly self-confident”. Responses were made on a 7 point scale ranging from (1) disagree very much to (7) completely agree.

  Detect Lies
Mean SD
Tell Lies
Mean SD
Believe others
Mean SD
Tell Truths
Mean SD
Prosecutors 75.2 (18.3) 41.7 (25.4) 58.4 (22.7) 76.3 (16.8)
Lay-people 62.8 (25.2) 41.9 (29.3) 62.2 (22.4) 71.6 (18.7)

 

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION

Table 1 presents the mean self-assessed abilities computed for prosecutors and lay people. A t-test for independent samples revealed that prosecutors assessed their lie detection ability significantly higher than laypeople, t (62) = 2.24, p =.028. The other three abilities were not significantly different. Using a paired sample t-tests, it was further found that unlike laypeople, prosecutors assessed their lie-detection ability significantly higher than their believing ability, t (31) = 3.08, p=004 (Table 1).

The highly assessed lie detection ability was further endorsed by the statement: “I am convinced that I can ascertain when a defendant is lying or is telling the truth”. Twenty five prosecutors agreed (ratings 5-7) while only one disagreed (ratings 1-3). Assuming that an unbiased response should be evenly distributed above and below the middle point of the scale (4), this distribution is extremely skewed. The tendency of prosecutors to rate high their lie-detection ability was also supported by the agreement (26 vs. 1) to the statement: “While cross-examining I am highly self-confident”. The lower assessed believing ability received support by prosecutors contesting (27 vs. 1) the statement: “Usually I tend to believe the defendant I interrogate”, and by disagreeing (23 vs. 3) that: “If I interrogate a defendant for a while and did not reach the conclusion that he is lying, he must be truthful”. This attitude contrasts the common “truth bias” or the tendency to judge messages as truthful rather than deceptive when in doubt [16].

People predominantly believe that liars act more nervously than truth tellers [12] and police officers are of no exception. However, prosecutors disagreed (21 vs. 6) that: “The nervous defendant is likely to be deceptive”. Another incorrect belief about deception cues is the notion that liars avoid eye contact [3]. Prosecutors disagreed (21 vs. 4) that: “During the interrogation, a defendant that does not look into my eyes is lying”.

There is evidence that interrogators who are convinced of the suspects’ guilt conduct pressure-filled interrogations to elicit a confession [15,17]. Applying this insight to criminal prosecutors revealed that they tended to agree (16 vs. 8) that: “When I am convinced that the defendant is deceptive I will make the extra effort to force him to confess.” and disagree (25 vs 2) with the statement: “I am afraid that if I will put pressure on the defendant he will confess to crimes he did not commit”.

Kassin [14] noted that police interrogators believe that they do not interrogate innocent suspects. The present prosecutors disagree. They disputed (29 vs. 1) the statement: “There is no such thing as innocent defendants”. However, they also disputed (30 vs 1) the statement: “There are many false convictions”.

Finally, Kassin [14] indicated that: “special training in deception detection may lead investigators to make prejudgments of guilt with confidence (p. 217). Prosecutors rejected the presumption of guilt and contested (26 vs. 4) the statement: “I tend to ascertain that the defendant is deceptive before I start to interrogate him”.

CONCLUSIONS

The current study was designed to provide an initial glance on the perceived abilities of criminal prosecutors to tell and detect lies, tell the truth convincingly and believe others. Results replicated previous findings as to the tendency of law enforcement personnel to overestimate their lie-detection ability which was explained by the self-assessing bias or the desire to avoid an appearance of being easily deceived and therefore unprofessional. This tendency may be supported by the absence of valid corrective feedback about the accuracy of their judgments because the legal truth is not necessarily the actual truth.

Truthful defense witnesses (including the innocent defendant) might experience difficulties in conveying their truths when cross-examined by criminal prosecutors dedicated to convictions, with self-confidence about their lie-detecting skills, who perceive their ability to believe others lower than their ability to detect lies. This is particularly true for insecure but truthful witnesses interrogated by prosecutors who are not afraid to pressurize the defendant to facilitate confessions. It may be suggested that the court should acknowledge the prosecutors’ conceptions, and adopt more caution towards testimonies of insecure witnesses.

The immediate critical argument that comes to mind is that prosecutors intentionally over-rated their lie-detecting ability for purposes of self-presentation as professionals, while in practice they did not know precisely where they stand compared to other people in their ability to detect deception. Williams and Gilovich [18] indicated otherwise. They showed that people truly believe in their high self-assessments and take their estimates seriously enough to guide their behavior.

REFERENCES

1. Bond CF Jr, DePaulo BM. Accuracy of deception judgments. Pers Soc Psychol Rev. 2006; 10: 214-234.

2. De Paulo BM, Pfeifer RL. On-the-job experience and skill at detecting deception. Journal of Applied Social Psychology.1986; 16: 249-267.

3. Vrij A, Semin GR. Lie experts’ beliefs about nonverbal indicators of deception. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior.1996; 20: 65-80.

4. Ekman P, O’Sullivan M. Who can catch a liar? Am Psychol. 1991; 46: 913-920.

5. Elaad E. Effects of feedback on the overestimated capacity to detect lies and the underestimated ability to tell lies. Applied Cognitive Psychology. 2003; 17: 349-363.

6. Elaad E. Lie-detection biases among male police interrogators, prisoners, and laypersons. Psychol Rep. 2009; 105:1047-1056.

7. Elaad E, Lavy S, Cohenca D, Berholz E, Thee P, and Ben-Gigi Y. Lies, truths, and attachment orientations in late adolescence. Personality and Individual Differences. 2012; 52: 670-673.

8. Elaad E. Effects of feedback on self-assessed and actual abilities to tell lies. In: Hasselm AE. editor. Crime: Causes, Types and Victims. Hauppauge (NY): Nova Science Publishers. 2011. pp. 155-166.

9. Gilovich T, Savitsky K, Medvec VH. The illusion of transparency: biased assessments of others’ ability to read one’s emotional states. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1998; 75: 332-346.

10. Tversky A, Kahneman D. Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases. Science. 1974; 185: 1124-1131.

11. .Kaplar ME, Gordon AK. The enigma of altruistic lying: Perspective differences in what motivates and justifies lie telling within romantic relationships. Personal Relationships.2004; 11: 489-507.

12. Vrij A. Detecting lies and deceit: Pitfalls and opportunities, 2nd Ed. Chichester UK: Wiley; 2008..

13. Stromwall LA, Granhag PA, and Hartwig M. Practitioners’ beliefs about deception. In PA Granhag and LA Stromwall, (Eds.), The Detection of Deception in Forensic Contexts. Cambridge UK: Cambridge University Press; 2004; 229-250.

14. Kassin SM. On the psychology of confessions: does innocence put innocents at risk? Am Psychol. 2005; 60: 215-228.

15. Kassin SM, Goldstein CC, Savitsky K. Behavioral confirmation in the interrogation room: on the dangers of presuming guilt. Law Hum Behav. 2003; 27: 187-203.

16. Kohnken G. (1989). Behavioral correlates of statement credibility: Theories, paradigms and results. In H Wegener, F Losel, and J Haisch (Eds.), Criminal behavior and the justice system: Psychological perspectives. New York: Springer-Verlag. 271-289.

17. Perillo JT, Kassin SM. Inside interrogation: the lie, the bluff, and false confessions. Law Hum Behav. 2011; 35: 327-337.

18. Williams EF, Gilovich T. Do people really believe they are above average? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 2008; 44: 1121- 1128

Received : 05 Mar 2015
Accepted : 19 Jun 2015
Published : 22 Jun 2015
Journals
Annals of Otolaryngology and Rhinology
ISSN : 2379-948X
Launched : 2014
JSM Schizophrenia
Launched : 2016
Journal of Nausea
Launched : 2020
JSM Internal Medicine
Launched : 2016
JSM Hepatitis
Launched : 2016
JSM Oro Facial Surgeries
ISSN : 2578-3211
Launched : 2016
Journal of Human Nutrition and Food Science
ISSN : 2333-6706
Launched : 2013
JSM Regenerative Medicine and Bioengineering
ISSN : 2379-0490
Launched : 2013
JSM Spine
ISSN : 2578-3181
Launched : 2016
Archives of Palliative Care
ISSN : 2573-1165
Launched : 2016
JSM Nutritional Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3203
Launched : 2017
Annals of Neurodegenerative Disorders
ISSN : 2476-2032
Launched : 2016
Journal of Fever
ISSN : 2641-7782
Launched : 2017
JSM Bone Marrow Research
ISSN : 2578-3351
Launched : 2016
JSM Mathematics and Statistics
ISSN : 2578-3173
Launched : 2014
Journal of Autoimmunity and Research
ISSN : 2573-1173
Launched : 2014
JSM Arthritis
ISSN : 2475-9155
Launched : 2016
JSM Head and Neck Cancer-Cases and Reviews
ISSN : 2573-1610
Launched : 2016
JSM General Surgery Cases and Images
ISSN : 2573-1564
Launched : 2016
JSM Anatomy and Physiology
ISSN : 2573-1262
Launched : 2016
JSM Dental Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1548
Launched : 2016
Annals of Emergency Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1017
Launched : 2016
Annals of Mens Health and Wellness
ISSN : 2641-7707
Launched : 2017
Journal of Preventive Medicine and Health Care
ISSN : 2576-0084
Launched : 2018
Journal of Chronic Diseases and Management
ISSN : 2573-1300
Launched : 2016
Annals of Vaccines and Immunization
ISSN : 2378-9379
Launched : 2014
JSM Heart Surgery Cases and Images
ISSN : 2578-3157
Launched : 2016
Annals of Reproductive Medicine and Treatment
ISSN : 2573-1092
Launched : 2016
JSM Brain Science
ISSN : 2573-1289
Launched : 2016
JSM Biomarkers
ISSN : 2578-3815
Launched : 2014
JSM Biology
ISSN : 2475-9392
Launched : 2016
Archives of Stem Cell and Research
ISSN : 2578-3580
Launched : 2014
Annals of Clinical and Medical Microbiology
ISSN : 2578-3629
Launched : 2014
JSM Pediatric Surgery
ISSN : 2578-3149
Launched : 2017
Journal of Memory Disorder and Rehabilitation
ISSN : 2578-319X
Launched : 2016
JSM Tropical Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2578-3165
Launched : 2016
JSM Head and Face Medicine
ISSN : 2578-3793
Launched : 2016
JSM Cardiothoracic Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1297
Launched : 2016
JSM Bone and Joint Diseases
ISSN : 2578-3351
Launched : 2017
JSM Bioavailability and Bioequivalence
ISSN : 2641-7812
Launched : 2017
JSM Atherosclerosis
ISSN : 2573-1270
Launched : 2016
Journal of Genitourinary Disorders
ISSN : 2641-7790
Launched : 2017
Journal of Fractures and Sprains
ISSN : 2578-3831
Launched : 2016
Journal of Autism and Epilepsy
ISSN : 2641-7774
Launched : 2016
Annals of Marine Biology and Research
ISSN : 2573-105X
Launched : 2014
JSM Health Education & Primary Health Care
ISSN : 2578-3777
Launched : 2016
JSM Communication Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3807
Launched : 2016
Annals of Musculoskeletal Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3599
Launched : 2016
Annals of Virology and Research
ISSN : 2573-1122
Launched : 2014
JSM Renal Medicine
ISSN : 2573-1637
Launched : 2016
Journal of Muscle Health
ISSN : 2578-3823
Launched : 2016
JSM Genetics and Genomics
ISSN : 2334-1823
Launched : 2013
JSM Anxiety and Depression
ISSN : 2475-9139
Launched : 2016
Clinical Journal of Heart Diseases
ISSN : 2641-7766
Launched : 2016
Annals of Medicinal Chemistry and Research
ISSN : 2378-9336
Launched : 2014
JSM Pain and Management
ISSN : 2578-3378
Launched : 2016
JSM Women's Health
ISSN : 2578-3696
Launched : 2016
Clinical Research in HIV or AIDS
ISSN : 2374-0094
Launched : 2013
Journal of Endocrinology, Diabetes and Obesity
ISSN : 2333-6692
Launched : 2013
Journal of Substance Abuse and Alcoholism
ISSN : 2373-9363
Launched : 2013
JSM Neurosurgery and Spine
ISSN : 2373-9479
Launched : 2013
Journal of Liver and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2379-0830
Launched : 2014
Journal of Drug Design and Research
ISSN : 2379-089X
Launched : 2014
JSM Clinical Oncology and Research
ISSN : 2373-938X
Launched : 2013
JSM Bioinformatics, Genomics and Proteomics
ISSN : 2576-1102
Launched : 2014
JSM Chemistry
ISSN : 2334-1831
Launched : 2013
Journal of Trauma and Care
ISSN : 2573-1246
Launched : 2014
JSM Surgical Oncology and Research
ISSN : 2578-3688
Launched : 2016
Annals of Food Processing and Preservation
ISSN : 2573-1033
Launched : 2016
Journal of Radiology and Radiation Therapy
ISSN : 2333-7095
Launched : 2013
JSM Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation
ISSN : 2578-3572
Launched : 2016
Annals of Clinical Pathology
ISSN : 2373-9282
Launched : 2013
Annals of Cardiovascular Diseases
ISSN : 2641-7731
Launched : 2016
Journal of Behavior
ISSN : 2576-0076
Launched : 2016
Annals of Clinical and Experimental Metabolism
ISSN : 2572-2492
Launched : 2016
Clinical Research in Infectious Diseases
ISSN : 2379-0636
Launched : 2013
JSM Microbiology
ISSN : 2333-6455
Launched : 2013
Journal of Urology and Research
ISSN : 2379-951X
Launched : 2014
Journal of Family Medicine and Community Health
ISSN : 2379-0547
Launched : 2013
Annals of Pregnancy and Care
ISSN : 2578-336X
Launched : 2017
JSM Cell and Developmental Biology
ISSN : 2379-061X
Launched : 2013
Annals of Aquaculture and Research
ISSN : 2379-0881
Launched : 2014
Clinical Research in Pulmonology
ISSN : 2333-6625
Launched : 2013
Journal of Immunology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2333-6714
Launched : 2013
JSM Biochemistry and Molecular Biology
ISSN : 2333-7109
Launched : 2013
Annals of Breast Cancer Research
ISSN : 2641-7685
Launched : 2016
Annals of Gerontology and Geriatric Research
ISSN : 2378-9409
Launched : 2014
Journal of Sleep Medicine and Disorders
ISSN : 2379-0822
Launched : 2014
JSM Burns and Trauma
ISSN : 2475-9406
Launched : 2016
Chemical Engineering and Process Techniques
ISSN : 2333-6633
Launched : 2013
Annals of Clinical Cytology and Pathology
ISSN : 2475-9430
Launched : 2014
JSM Allergy and Asthma
ISSN : 2573-1254
Launched : 2016
Journal of Neurological Disorders and Stroke
ISSN : 2334-2307
Launched : 2013
Annals of Sports Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2379-0571
Launched : 2014
JSM Sexual Medicine
ISSN : 2578-3718
Launched : 2016
Annals of Vascular Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2378-9344
Launched : 2014
JSM Biotechnology and Biomedical Engineering
ISSN : 2333-7117
Launched : 2013
Journal of Hematology and Transfusion
ISSN : 2333-6684
Launched : 2013
JSM Environmental Science and Ecology
ISSN : 2333-7141
Launched : 2013
Journal of Cardiology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2333-6676
Launched : 2013
JSM Nanotechnology and Nanomedicine
ISSN : 2334-1815
Launched : 2013
Journal of Ear, Nose and Throat Disorders
ISSN : 2475-9473
Launched : 2016
JSM Ophthalmology
ISSN : 2333-6447
Launched : 2013
Journal of Pharmacology and Clinical Toxicology
ISSN : 2333-7079
Launched : 2013
Annals of Psychiatry and Mental Health
ISSN : 2374-0124
Launched : 2013
Medical Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology
ISSN : 2333-6439
Launched : 2013
Annals of Pediatrics and Child Health
ISSN : 2373-9312
Launched : 2013
JSM Clinical Pharmaceutics
ISSN : 2379-9498
Launched : 2014
JSM Foot and Ankle
ISSN : 2475-9112
Launched : 2016
JSM Alzheimer's Disease and Related Dementia
ISSN : 2378-9565
Launched : 2014
Journal of Addiction Medicine and Therapy
ISSN : 2333-665X
Launched : 2013
Journal of Veterinary Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2378-931X
Launched : 2013
Annals of Public Health and Research
ISSN : 2378-9328
Launched : 2014
Annals of Orthopedics and Rheumatology
ISSN : 2373-9290
Launched : 2013
Journal of Clinical Nephrology and Research
ISSN : 2379-0652
Launched : 2014
Annals of Community Medicine and Practice
ISSN : 2475-9465
Launched : 2014
Annals of Biometrics and Biostatistics
ISSN : 2374-0116
Launched : 2013
JSM Clinical Case Reports
ISSN : 2373-9819
Launched : 2013
Journal of Cancer Biology and Research
ISSN : 2373-9436
Launched : 2013
Journal of Surgery and Transplantation Science
ISSN : 2379-0911
Launched : 2013
Journal of Dermatology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2373-9371
Launched : 2013
JSM Gastroenterology and Hepatology
ISSN : 2373-9487
Launched : 2013
Annals of Nursing and Practice
ISSN : 2379-9501
Launched : 2014
JSM Dentistry
ISSN : 2333-7133
Launched : 2013
Author Information X