Loading

Annals of Forensic Research and Analysis

Human Variation in Face Aging in Adult Monozygotic Twins: Biometric Implications for the Forensic Sciences

Research Article | Open Access | Volume 3 | Issue 1

  • 1. Department of Anthropology, University of North Carolina Wilmington, USA
  • 2. Department of Chemistry, University of North Carolina Chapel Hill, USA
+ Show More - Show Less
Corresponding Authors
A Midori Albert, Department of Anthropology, University of North Carolina Wilmington, 601 S. College Road, Wilmington, NC, 28403, USA, Tel: 910-962-7078; Fax: 910- 962-3543
Abstract

This study examined the extent of similarity and divergence in facial appearance of adult monozygotic (i.e., genetically identical) twins as a function of age. Variation in face aging is relevant to advancing technologies in computer automated face recognition and age progression in the forensic sciences. The focus was on the influence that epigenetics had, if any, on the divergence of facial similarity, assessed by evaluating facial dimensions of adult monozygotic twins. The sample comprised high - resolution digital images of 65 twin sets aged 18 to 78 years, male and female, separated into three age groups (young, middle, and older adult). A reference sample of sub adult monozygotic twins (aged 6 to 18 years) was used for comparison. The digital images were landmarked and measurements were taken for several dimensions of the face. The data were analyzed to determine what dimensions of the face may show significant divergence as identical twins age. Results suggest that the role of epigenetics in face aging remains unclear; there was a high degree of variation with some twin sets’ facial appearance being more similar, while others showed significant divergence, seemingly independent of adult age.

Keywords

 Face aging; Facial appearance variation; Monozygotic twin variation; Epigenetics; Forensic science.

CITATION

Albert AM, Lloyd CC (2016) Human Variation in Face Aging in Adult Monozygotic Twins: Biometric Implications for the Forensic Sciences. Ann Forensic Res Anal 3(1): 1027.

INTRODUCTION

Intrinsic (biological) and extrinsic (environmental) factors are known to significantly affect human variation in face aging, and an understanding of these factors has, over the years, contributed to the development of, and improvements in, computer automated face recognition and facial age progression technologies in the forensic sciences [1]. There are definite genetic markers that affect face aging [2,3], and the phenomenon of epigenetics, or the compilation of genetic mutations that occur in DNA as an individual ages, may play a role in the changing appearance of the aging face. Epigenetic modifications result in miniscule changes in gene expression, which can change anything from one’s appearance to the way an individual behaves.

It was not until 2002 that the importance of epigenetics in twins was demonstrated [4]. Before 2002, monozygotic twins were believed to have identical DNA throughout their lifetimes; and any discrepancy in phenotypic appearance was considered a direct result of environment. However, recent research has shown that monozygotic twins have identical DNA when they are born, yet they develop and accumulate different DNA strands as they age [4-6]. Epigenetics can help explain why there may be variability in identical twins and how facial features of such twins may diverge in similarity of appearance over time.

Inasmuch as computer automated face recognition and age progression research endeavors to understand patterns in face aging to improve accuracy, the extent to which epigenetics could be involved in face aging is key. This was the impetus behind this present study. The approach to this research question was based in part on earlier research conducted on subadult monozygotic twins. Naini and Moss [7] studied the effects of epigenetics on the facial development in subadult monozygotic twins (6-18 years old). By way of understanding the relevance of epigenetic factors on subadult monozygotic twins, and that epigenetic factors have more of an effect on the DNA over time (i.e., as individuals age), due to the length of time that has passed allowing these epigenetic factors to set in, then it can be assumed that these factors will appear more pronounced in adult identical twins.

The present study involved an examination of a sample of face images of adult identical twins (n = 130) to determine whether epigenetic factors could play a role in the divergence of facial similarity over time, where it was hypothesized that twins would appear more dissimilar the older they become.

Hypotheses were developed for areas of the face that may retain similarity as to those that may be expected to change, based on a review of the literature [1,7] and anecdotal evidence of the aging face. The area that was hypothesized to show the most similarity between identical twins was the triangular area demarcating the eyes and nose, based on the Naini and Moss study [7]. It was hypothesized that the same measurements that showed statistically significant differences in subadult twin sets (i.e., deviation in facial similarity) would also show differences in adult twin sets, that certain facial dimensions that deviated in subadults would continue to deviate as twin’s age across the adult lifespan. It was also hypothesized that other areas of the face would continue to diverge as adult twin’s age, and that they would show statistically significant deviations at later stages in life due to degenerative changes and where epigenetic factors have a chance to establish themselves (and accumulate) in individuals.

It was hypothesized that the following features would show statistically significant differences with aging (please refer to Table (1) and Figure (1)):

Table 1: Landmark - based measurements used to analyze facial similarities and differences.

Measurement

 

Landmarks

 

Description of Landmarks

 

1

 

0-1

Right Eye Lateral Canthus to Right Eye Medial Canthus

2

 

1-2

Right Eye Medial Canthus to Soft Tissue Nasion

3

0-2

Right Eye Lateral Canthus to Soft Tissue Nasion

4

 

2-3

Soft Tissue Nasion to Left Eye Medial Canthus

5

3-4

Left Eye Medial Canthus to Left Eye Lateral Canthus

6

 

2-4

Soft Tissue Nasion to Left Eye Lateral Canthus

7

0-4

Right Eye Lateral Canthus to Left Eye Lateral Canthus

8

2-7

Soft Tissue Nasion to Pronasale

9

2-8

Soft Tissue Nasion to Subnasale

10

7-8

Pronasale to Subnasale

11

5-6

Right Alar Base to Left Alar Base

12

5-7

Right Alar Base to Pronasale

13

6-7

Left Alar Base to Pronasale

14

5-8

Right Alar Base to Subnasale

15

6-8

Left Alar Base to Subnasale

16

8-9

Subnasale to LabialeSuperius

17

9-10

LabialeSuperius to Stomion

18

10-11

Stomion to LabialeInferius

19

9-11

LabialeSuperius to LabialeInferius

20

11-12

LabialeInferius to Sublabiale

21

10-12

Stomion to Sublabiale

22

12-13

Sublabiale to Soft Tissue Pogonion

23

8-13

Subnasale to Soft Tissue Pogonion

24

2-13

Soft Tissue Nasion to Soft Tissue Pogonion

25

14-15

Right Cheilion to Left Cheilion

26

0-13

Right Eye Lateral Canthus to Soft Tissue Pogonion

27

4-13

Left Eye Lateral Canthus to Soft Tissue Pogonion

28

2-14

Soft Tissue Nasion to Right Cheilion

29

2-15

Soft Tissue Nasion to Left Cheilion

Figure 1: The 29 dimensions taken between landmarks [8].

1. Right Alar Base to Left Alar Base

2. Soft Tissue Nasion to Subnasale

3. Subnasale to Labiale Inferius

4. Labiale Superius to Stomion

5. Labiale Inferius to Stomion

6. Labiale Superius to Labiale Inferius

7. Left Cheilion to Right Cheilion

8. Stomion to Sublabiale

9. Sublabiale to Soft Tissue Pogonion

It was hypothesized that the following measurements would show no major divergence between twins in a twin set (please refer to Table (1) and Figure (1)):

1. Lateral to Medial Canthus of Left Eye

2. Medial Canthus of Left Eye to Soft Tissue Nasion

3. Soft Tissue Nasion to Medial Canthus of Right Eye

4. Soft Tissue Nasion to Pronasale

5. Right Alar Base to Subnasale

6. Soft Tissue Nasion to Right Alar Base

7. Soft Tissue Nasion to Left Alar Base

8. Left Alar Base to Subnasale

The areas that were expected to show no statistically significant deviation have been found in previous studies to remain fairly constant (similar) throughout the adult lifespan [1,7]. Features such as inter orbital width do not change much in individuals, for example.

The objective of this study was to examine if any divergences in facial similarity could be measured as a function of age in adult monozygotic twins, who share the same genetic makeup, to better understand what role epigenetics may play in face aging.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Sample

The sample for this study comprised 65 sets (or 130 digital images) of identical twin faces, which contained one image of each twin per twin set. The twin faces are part of the collection of biometric images labeled “ND – Twins -2009-2010”, and are part of the UND Biometrics Database [9]. Of the 65 twin face sets, 13 were male and 52 were female. Since the sample sizes were too disparate between the sexes to get meaningful results, sex differences in aging patterns were not assessed.

The sample was split into three different age categories. Age Group 1 contained 17 twin face sets and ranged from 18-27 years old (to encompass individuals in the 20’s). Age Group 2 was composed of 25 twin sets ranging from 37-51 years old (to represent those in the decade of the 40’s). Age group 3 contained 23 twin sets that were 54-78 years old (to embody those that are age 50 years and older). The delineation for the age groups was based on where the most notable diachronic changes take place in an individual’s lifetime [1]. By separating the sample into the above three age groupings, any patterns of facial similarity, or divergence of similarity, in twin sets that may be tied to an age - effect could be shown. Since aging is not necessarily a steady process, the age ranges chosen for this study help to emphasize decades where major changes typically occur in face aging throughout an individual’s lifetime.

A reference sample was used to make comparisons between subadult and adult monozygotic twins. The face aging data from the above three age groups were compared to results of a study of facial similarity and divergence in subadult (6-18 years old) monozygotic twins conducted by Naini and Moss [7]. Naini and Moss [7] looked at changes in facial appearance of subadult monozygotic twins during growth and development, whereas the present study looked at the effects the aging process on adult monozygotic twins, essentially changes occurring over time, after growth and development has been completed. The sample for this study and the reference sample from Naini and Moss’s [7] study on subadult monozygotic twins can be seen in Table (2).

Table 2: Sample of adult faces (present study) and reference sample of subadult faces (Naini and Moss [7]).

Age (Years Old)

 

# of Males

# of Females

Sub Adults (6-18)

 

5 twin pairs

5 twin pairs

Young Adults (18-27)

 

6 twin pairs

11 twin pairs

Middle - Aged Adults (37-51)

 

4 twin pairs

21 twin pairs

Older Adults (54-78)

 

3 twin pairs

20 twin pairs

Data collection: Land marking

In order to determine if there were areas of the face that deviate significantly in similarity of appearance in adult monozygotic twins over the course of the lifespan, measures of various facial dimensions were obtained (i.e., eye width, nose width, etc.). The approach to obtain and ensure accurate measures was to select a set of landmarks and “mark up” each face in the sample, meaning that for each digital image, fixed points on the image were added and this is known as “land marking” or “marking up” a face. Landmarks are standardized fixed points that are determined by anatomical features and are used in craniometric analyses. Each point on the digital facial images represents a coordinate in 2 - dimensional space with both an x and a y position. The face images were standardized at the time the photographs were taken. The landmarks for this study were well known and widely used because they represent key areas of the face used in measures of various dimensions to replicate face shape quantitatively. The landmarks can also be used in statistical analyses to help in sex or population classification in forensic cases of unknown human identity, for example.

The landmarks for the present study are identical to those in Naini and Moss [7] to allow for comparisons between the twin data sets - subadult and adult. Comparisons of the subadults and adults would take into account the notion that variation in facial dimensions in subadult twins is likely due to the processes of growth and development, whereas variation in facial dimensions in adult twins, who have completed growth and development, is likely due to “aging” in a degenerative sense. Once specific landmarks were chosen, a computer software program known as Face mark v. 2.0 [10] was used to overlay pinpoints on the digital images in the sample. “Marking - up” images involved placing landmarks on digital photographs of faces in the database. All 65 pairs of identical twins were marked up similarly. The landmarks begin with the number 0 and proceed to 15 sequentially as shown in Table (3) and Figure (2).

Table 3: Landmark names and descriptions.

Landmark

Feature

Region/Description

0

Right Eye Lateral Canthus

Outside Corner of Right Eye

1

Right Eye Medial Canthus

Inside Corner of Right Eye

2

Soft Tissue Nasion

Area on Nose Directly Between Pupils

3

Left Eye Medial Canthus

Inside Corner of Left Eye

4

Left Eye Lateral Canthus

Outside Corner of Left Eye

5

Right Alar Base

Farthest Point on Right of Nose

6

Left Alar Base

Farthest Point on Left of Nose

7

Pronasale

Most Anterior Point on Nose

8

Subnasale

Point Where Nose Meets "Upper Lip"

9

LabialeSuperius

Point Where Upper Lip Truly Begins

10

Stomion

Point Directly Where Upper and Lower Lip Meet

11

LabialeInferius

Point Where Lower Lip Truly Ends

12

Sublabiale

Point of Maximum Concavity on Lower Lip

13

Soft Tissue Pogonion

Most Anterior Point on Chin

14

Right Cheilion

Right Corner of Mouth Where Lips Connect

15

Left Cheilion

Left Corner of Mouth Where Lips Connect

Figure 2: Example of landmarks on individuals in twin database [8].

Data collection: Measurements

Measurements were taken between different selected pairs of landmarks and represent different facial feature dimensions. These measures were analyzed to determine where differences might exist between twin pairs in the sample. The dimensions selected were made for ease of comparison of adults in this study to findings of subadults from Naini and Moss [7]. The measurements used are listed in Table (1) and illustrated in Figure (1).

A Python script (i.e., computer program) was written to transfer the data from Face mark v. 2.0 to a Microsoft Excel file, via a comma - separated variables file. The data were then organized and the measurements were taken as described above. Microsoft Excel was used to perform statistical testing of the data. Paired samples t - tests were run to determine if there were any statistically significant differences between the measurements for each age group and for all measures of facial dimensions between each twin, termed Twin A and Twin B, in each twin set. Findings from these statistical tests are discussed next.

Several statistical tests were employed to examine the data in different ways. At first, paired samples t - tests were performed for each measurement of a facial dimension for each age group, separately. For example, Measurement 1 was tested for all individuals in Age Group 1 to determine if Twin A and Twin B of a given twin set yielded results that were statistically significantly different.

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION

The measurements that showed statistically significant differences (p < .05) were Measurement 17 for Age Group 3, Measurement 19 for Age Group 3, and Measurement 21 for Age Group 1. Measurement 17 was the distance between the labialesuperius and the stomion (upper lip and space between lips, or upper lip thickness). Measurement 19 was the distance between the labialesuperius and the labialeinferius (upper and lower lips, or the thickness of both lips). Measurement 21 was the distance between the stomion and the sublabiale (space between lips and point of maximum concavity on lower lip, or lower lip thickness). The t - test results for Measurements 17, 19, and 21 can be seen in Tables (4-6) respectively (p < .05).

Table 4: t-test for Measurement 17, Age Group 3.

t-Test: Paired Two Sample for Means

 

Variable 1

Variable 2

Mean

11.62478826

13.41744106

Variance

15.9199281

16.06252316

Observations

23

23

Pearson Correlation

0.645911253

 

Hypothesized Mean Difference

0

 

df

22

 

t Stat

-2.554725415

 

P(T <= t) one-tail

0.009032093

 

t Critical one-tail

1.717144374

 

P(T <= t) two-tail

0.018064187

 

t Critical two-tail

2.073873068

 

Table 5: t-test for Measurement 19, Age Group 3.

t-Test: Paired Two Sample for Means

 

Variable 1

Variable 2

Mean

24.20655866

27.79174636

Variance

60.5335868

65.21228698

Observations

23

23

Pearson Correlation

0.731043655

 

Hypothesized Mean Difference

0

 

df

22

 

t Stat

-2.953790823

 

P(T <= t) one-tail

0.003669172

 

t Critical one-tail

1.717144374

 

P(T <= t) two-tail

0.007338344

 

t Critical two-tail

2.073873068

 

Table 6: t-test for Measurement 21, Age Group 1.

t-Test: Paired Two Sample for Means

 
 

Variable 1

Variable 2

Mean

32.25540857

30.5421093

Variance

9.356150216

10.95077763

Observations

17

17

Pearson Correlation

0.526559092

 

Hypothesized Mean Difference

0

 

df

16

 

t Stat

2.274354675

 

P(T <= t) one-tail

0.01853248

 

t Critical one-tail

1.745883676

 

P(T <= t) two-tail

0.03706496

 

t Critical two-tail

2.119905299

 

Results of statistical testing did not support the hypothesis that identical twins would continue to show divergence in similarity in the face throughout their lifetime. Each twin set was given a number (starting with 1 and proceeding through 65) and each twin in each of the 65 pairs was labeled Twin A or Twin B. A paired samples t - test was run for each twin pair (1 65) to determine whether or not the measurements from Twin A to Twin B within a twin set were statistically different or not. Table (8) in the Appendix gives the results obtained from the paired sample t -tests. Overall, 26 twin pairs were statistically significantly different (p < .05) from one another while 39 twin pairs did not show statistically significant differences. For Age Group 1, seven twin pairs were statistically significantly different (p < .05) while ten twin pairs did not show statistically significant differences (41%). For Age Group 2, nine twin pairs were statistically significantly different (p < .05) from one another while 16 twin pairs did not show statistically significant differences (36%). For Age Group 3, ten twin pairs showed statistically significant differences (p < .05) while 13 twin pairs were not statistically significantly different from one another (44%). Of the 13 male twin sets, seven showed statistically significant differences (p < .05) while six showed no statisticallysignificant differences (54%). Of the 52 female twin pairs, 19 showed statistically significant differences (p < .05) while 33 did not show any statistically significant differences (36%). Table (7) below shows the percent difference for each age group.

Table 7: Results of paired samples t-tests comparing Twin A to Twin B within a given twin set for all facial dimensions.

Age Group

 

Sample Size

 

(Twin Sets)

 

Number of Twin Sets with Significant Differences in Facial Measures

 

Percentage of Twin Sets with Significant Differences in Facial Measures

 

1

 

17

 

7

 

41

 

2

 

25

 

9

 

36

 

3

 

23

 

10

 

44

A Pearson’s correlation was also run for the 65 twin sets to determine if twin sets at an older age had measurements that yielded a lower correlation between Twin A and Twin B than twin sets at a younger age, which could suggest greater divergence (less similarity) in facial appearance. If older twin sets had a lower correlation compared to younger twin sets, this would suggest that the measured dimensions between the landmarks deviate more as adult monozygotic twins age. However, the Pearson correlation was high (0.98-0.99) for all 65 twin sets, regardless of age. Interpretations of the findings are discussed presently.

Analysis of t-tests comparing facial dimensions among Age Groups 1, 2, and 3

The sample was divided into three age groups: Age Group 1 included young adults (18-27 years old) encompassing the effects of aging on twins in their individual measure of a facial dimension. There was one dimension that was found to be statistically significantly different (p < .05) for Age Group 1, the youngest age group, and two dimensions that were found to be statistically significantly different (p < .05) for Age Group 3, the oldest age group. All other measures did not show any statistically significant differences between Twin A and Twin B of a twin pair.

Measurement 21: stomion to sublabiale

Measurement 21 was found to be significantly different (p < .05) for Age Group 1. Measurement 21 was the distance between the stomion and the sublabiale; this is where the upper and lower lips meet to the point of maximum concavity on the lower lip (see Figure (2)). It is important to note that this dimension is statistically different for twins in Age Group 1, the youngest age group, but not for Age Groups 2 or 3, which were composed of older individuals. This suggests that the difference in this measurement becomes less pronounced over the course of an adult’s lifespan, which is counter to the hypothesis that greater differences are evident due to advancing age. This could be explained by the ability to easily distort the lip regions by facial expression, which would lead to a difference in the appearance of faces in a photograph.

Measurement 17: labialesuperius to stomion

Measurement 17 was found to show a statistically significant difference (p < .05) for Age Group 3. Measurement 17 was the difference between the labialesuperius and the stomion; this refers to the distance between the point where the upper lip truly begins, to the point where the upper and lower lips meet, or the thickness of the upper lip (see Figure (2)).

Measurement 19: labialesuperius to labialeinferius

Measurement 19 was the second of two measures found statistically significantly different (p < .05) for Age Group 3. Measurement 19 was the distance from the labialesuperius to the labialeinferius; this is the distance between where the upper lip truly begins to where the lower lip truly ends, or the thickness of the upper and lower lips combined (see Figure (2)). Significant differences could be due to a difference in BMI, differences in water retention, or a difference in facial expression between twins. Thinning of the lips also generally occurs as adults age, which could explain the difference in this dimension between twins of different age groups, similar to the finding for Measurement 17.

Results of the above analyses were compared to findings from the study conducted on subadult monozygotic twin facial similarity by Naini and Moss [7]. Naini and Moss [7] obtained their face data by taking 3 - dimensional facial scans of subadult monozygotic twins. This method may have allowed for more sensitive data analysis when compared to the present study’s method of land marking 2 - dimensional digital face images from a database.

Although the images for the present study were standardized, there was the possibility of greater error, associated with placing the landmarks on faces, and then taking measurements when compared to taking a 3 - dimensional facial scan of an individual. Moreover, it is possible that the 2-D data be less sensitive to statistical analyses when compared to 3-D surface facial scans. It is also possible that the selected measures may not translate to the differences seen visually. This means that the human eye may be picking up on differences in facial features that are qualitatively different between twin sets and that the quantitative features (the measured dimensions) are not features adequately represent facial similarity or dissimilarity.

Naini and Moss [7] compared 3-D facial surface shape analysis scans among and between pairs of subadult (ages 6-18 years old) monozygotic twins. Their study using facial scans was conducted for two reasons: to see if facial scans supported inter landmark measurements taken between each twin within a twin set (similar to this study) and to compare areas of the face that differentiated monozygotic twin sets. Two scans were taken for each individual and then averaged together to avoid any error in intra - examiner reproducibility. Naini and Moss [7] found that the “greatest genetic determination of appearance seems to be the mid facial area between the lateral orbital rims and the nose.” They also found that the lips, mouth, and cheeks are not very similar. This finding is consistent with findings from this study, which showed statistically significant differences in the lip region. This suggests that this area of the face is either dissimilar between monozygotic twins in general or it is easily distorted by a change in facial expression. Other extrinsic factors including BMI, use of makeup, water retention, etc., which can alter facial appearance, may help explain the statistically significant differences observed in the labiale (lip) region.

Analysis of t-tests comparing Twin A to Twin B in each twin set using all measures of facial dimensions

For the second set of paired samples t - tests, all of the facial dimensions for Twin A were compared to all of the dimensions for Twin B for all 65 twin sets as adult age increases. Twin A and Twin B represent two separate twins that belong to the same twin set. The goal was to determine if there is greater deviation in facial appearance due to age throughout the lifetime of adult twins. This was done by using paired samples t - tests; statistically significant differences would support the hypothesis that older adult twins would show more evidence of deviation in facial appearance when compared to young adult twins and especially when compared to the subadult monozygotic twins in the reference sample [7]. In Age Group 1, the youngest age group (18-27 years old), 7 out of 17 twin sets showed statistically significant differences (p < .05) between Twin A and Twin B in a given twin set (41%). In Age Group 2, middle-aged adults (37-51 years old), 9 out of 25 twin sets yielded statistically significant differences (p < .05) between Twin A and Twin B in a given twin set (36%). For Age Group 3, the oldest age group, 10 out of 23 twin sets showed statistically significant differences (p < .05) between Twin A and Twin B for a given twin set (44%). The findings for these t - tests are shown in Table (7). Table (8) shows the same information, but with the sample in greater detail with ages and sexes of twin sets listed.

Table 8: t-test analyses of individual twin sets.

Twin Set

Age

Sex

Age Group

Statistically Significant Results (Divergence in Facial Appearance) in Twin A and Twin B from Paired Samples t-tests

1

 

18

F

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

1

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

No

2

 

18

M

No

3

 

18

F

No

4

 

18

M

Yes

5

 

18

F

No

6

 

19

F

Yes

7

 

19

M

Yes

8

 

19

F

No

9

 

20

M

No

10

 

21

F

Yes

11

 

22

M

Yes

12

 

23

F

No

13

 

24

M

No

14

 

24

F

Yes

15

 

26

F

No

16

 

26

F

No

17

 

27

F

Yes

18

 

37

F

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

2

No

19

 

37

F

No

20

 

38

F

No

21

 

38

M

No

22

 

39

F

Yes

23

 

41

F

No

24

 

41

F

No

25

 

41

F

Yes

26

 

41

F

No

27

 

42

F

No

28

 

42

F

Yes

29

 

42

F

No

30

 

44

F

No

31

 

45

F

No

32

 

46

F

Yes

33

 

47

F

No

34

 

47

M

Yes

35

 

47

F

Yes

36

 

48

F

No

37

 

49

F

Yes

38

 

49

F

No

39

 

49

F

No

40

 

50

F

Yes

41

 

51

M

2

Yes

42

 

51

M

 

No

43

 

54

F

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

3

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Yes

44

 

54

F

 

Yes

45

 

55

M

 

No

46

 

55

F

 

No

47

 

55

F

 

Yes

48

 

56

F

 

No

49

 

56

F

 

No

50

 

56

F

 

No

51

 

60

F

 

No

52

 

61

F

 

No

53

 

62

F

 

No

54

 

63

F

 

Yes

55

 

65

F

 

No

56

 

66

F

 

Yes

57

 

67

F

 

No

58

 

69

F

 

No

59

 

69

F

 

No

60

 

72

M

 

No

61

 

72

F

 

Yes

62

 

74

F

 

Yes

63

 

76

M

 

Yes

64

 

77

F

 

Yes

65

 

78

F

 

Yes

The percent difference is roughly the same for all three age groups; there is no suggestion that twins’ faces in older twin sets are more distinguishable than the younger twin sets. No patterns connecting a change in facial appearance to age could be established based on the given results of the paired sample t - tests. These test results reflected a difference in twins’ facial appearance in general, regardless of age. These results, when compared to findings from Naini and Moss [7], seem to support the interpretation that epigenetic factors perhaps have a stronger influence on facial appearance at younger ages, during the processes of growth and development, whereas environmental factors seem more influential at older ages.

One of the main goals of this study was to identify the role that epigenetics may have on the facial appearance of adult twins’ faces as a function of age. Based on the analyses it appears that epigenetics does not seem to have a measurable effect on face aging in adult identical twins. While epigenetics involves minute mutations that change the overall structure of DNA, they do not seem to have a major impact on facial appearance at least where 2-dimensional facial measurements are concerned as aging progresses in adult monozygotic twins. Epigenetics could play a minor role in facial appearance as a function of aging. However, epigenetic factors may only affect certain individuals in certain cases and may not be measurable in the same way in all people.

Figures (3-5) (with twin sets lettered a-l for ease in referencing) are examples of twin sets that do and do not show statistically significant differences in facial dimensions between Twin A and Twin B. As seen in the photographs, some of the images that resulted in statistically significant differences (p < .05) for the paired samples t - tests are of faces that look very similar to the human eye (Figure 3a,3b,4e,4f,5i,5j), while some of the images that did not show any statistically significant differences are of faces that look quite dissimilar to the human eye (Figure 3c,3d,4g,4h).

Figure 3: Age Group 1 Twin Sets (a-b, c-d) [9].

Naini and Moss [7] suggest that genetic and environmental changes influence the shape of the face and changes in dimension between twins. This study suggests that epigenetic factors do not seem to have a discernable effect on 2-dimensional facial measurements in adult twin sets of various ages.

CONCLUSION

Paired sample t-tests were run to compare different facial dimensions among and between the three different age groups. Measurement 17 and Measurement 19 were found to be statistically significantly different for Age Group 3. Measurement 21 showed a statistically significant difference for Age Group 1 but not for Age Group 2 or Age Group 3. All three measurements that showed statistically significant differences were found in the labiale (lip) region of the face, which can be easily altered by facial expression and other extrinsic factors such as body mass index (BMI), water retention, and use of makeup.

The second set of paired sample t - tests compared all of the dimensions of Twin A to all of the dimensions of Twin B in a twin set, for all 65 twin sets. The percentage of statistically significantly different twin sets for all three age groups was roughly the same which implies that epigenetics does not have a major measurable effect on certain facial measures as adult monozygotic twins age. However, epigenetics could play a minor role in the differences in facial appearance between each twin from a given twin set; developing a method to detect this is key. Figures (4-6) help show that the human eye is still superior in terms of detecting facial similarities and dissimilarities.

Figure 4: Age Group 2 Twin Sets (a-b, c-d) [9].

Figure 5: Age Group 3 Twin Sets (a-b) [9].

The limitations to this study were centered upon the methods both in data collection and in analysis. Data collection was performed on 2-dimensional digital images from a database; it was not possible to obtain 3-dimensional facial scans of the same individuals. Manual placement of the landmarks on each individual twin facial image could have resulted in minor errors in the results. It is also likely that the facial measures selected simply do not show differences that can be detected quantitatively something that human perception is better picking up on. Evidence from this study supports that computer automated facial recognition technologies benefit more from focusing on facial features that tend to remain constant, rather than those that change, with increasing adult age.

When combined with other biometrics, computer automated face recognition technologies work fairly well. However, by itself, as with any other biometric, there are limitations. Research in the forensic sciences geared toward biomertrics aims to synthezie and integrates the various methods available, such as fingerprinting, iris detection, voice and gait identification, and face recognition to improve security and accuracy in the authentication and verification of human identity. By understanding that epigenetics does not currently have a detectable measureable effect on adult face aging, new knowledge is brought to the forensic sciences that informs the next steps in research in this growing area.

REFERENCES

1. Albert AM, Ricanek K Jr, Patterson E. A review of the literature on the aging adult skull and face: Implications for forensic science research and applications. Forensic Sci Intl. 2007; 172: 1-9.

2. Guyuron B, Rowe DJ, Weinfeld AB, Eshraghi Y, Fathi A, Iamphongsai Cite this article S. Factors contributing to the facial aging of identical twins. Plast Reconstr Surg. 2009; 123: 1321-1331.

3. Tammen SA, Friso S, Choi SW. Epigenetics: The link between nature and nurture. Mol Aspects Med. 2013; 34: 753-764.

4. Singh SM, Murphy B, O’Reilly R. Epigenetic contributors to the discordance of monozygotic twins. Clin Genet. 2002; 62: 97-103.

5. Fraga M, Ballestar E, Paz M, Ropero S, Setien F, Ballestar M, et al. Epigenetic differences arise during the lifetime of monozygotic twins. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2005; 102: 10604-10609.

6. PetronisA, Gottesman II, Kan P, Kennedy J, Basile V, Paterson AD, et al. Monozygotic Twins Exhibit Numerous Epigenetic Differences: Clues to Twin Discordance? Schizophr Bull. 2003; 29: 169-178.

7. Naini FB, Moss JP. Three-dimensional assessment of the relative contribution of genetics and environment to various facial parameters with the twin method. Am J Orthod Dentofacial Orthop. 2004; 126: 655-665.

8. Ngaladel. The Face Sketch. Digital image. DeviantART. 2012; (last accessed February 18, 2016).

9. Phillips PJ, Flynn P, Bower K, VorderBruegge R, Grother P, Quinn G, et al. Distinguishing identical twins by face recognition.

10. Barbour B, Ricanek K. An interactive tool for extremely dense landmarking of faces. Proceedings of the 1st International Workshop on Visual Interfaces for Ground Truth Collection. 2012; 13: 1-5.

Albert AM, Lloyd CC (2016) Human Variation in Face Aging in Adult Monozygotic Twins: Biometric Implications for the Forensic Sciences. Ann Forensic Res Anal 3(1): 1027.

Received : 12 Jul 2016
Accepted : 03 Aug 2016
Published : 04 Aug 2016
Journals
Annals of Otolaryngology and Rhinology
ISSN : 2379-948X
Launched : 2014
JSM Schizophrenia
Launched : 2016
Journal of Nausea
Launched : 2020
JSM Internal Medicine
Launched : 2016
JSM Hepatitis
Launched : 2016
JSM Oro Facial Surgeries
ISSN : 2578-3211
Launched : 2016
Journal of Human Nutrition and Food Science
ISSN : 2333-6706
Launched : 2013
JSM Regenerative Medicine and Bioengineering
ISSN : 2379-0490
Launched : 2013
JSM Spine
ISSN : 2578-3181
Launched : 2016
Archives of Palliative Care
ISSN : 2573-1165
Launched : 2016
JSM Nutritional Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3203
Launched : 2017
Annals of Neurodegenerative Disorders
ISSN : 2476-2032
Launched : 2016
Journal of Fever
ISSN : 2641-7782
Launched : 2017
JSM Bone Marrow Research
ISSN : 2578-3351
Launched : 2016
JSM Mathematics and Statistics
ISSN : 2578-3173
Launched : 2014
Journal of Autoimmunity and Research
ISSN : 2573-1173
Launched : 2014
JSM Arthritis
ISSN : 2475-9155
Launched : 2016
JSM Head and Neck Cancer-Cases and Reviews
ISSN : 2573-1610
Launched : 2016
JSM General Surgery Cases and Images
ISSN : 2573-1564
Launched : 2016
JSM Anatomy and Physiology
ISSN : 2573-1262
Launched : 2016
JSM Dental Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1548
Launched : 2016
Annals of Emergency Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1017
Launched : 2016
Annals of Mens Health and Wellness
ISSN : 2641-7707
Launched : 2017
Journal of Preventive Medicine and Health Care
ISSN : 2576-0084
Launched : 2018
Journal of Chronic Diseases and Management
ISSN : 2573-1300
Launched : 2016
Annals of Vaccines and Immunization
ISSN : 2378-9379
Launched : 2014
JSM Heart Surgery Cases and Images
ISSN : 2578-3157
Launched : 2016
Annals of Reproductive Medicine and Treatment
ISSN : 2573-1092
Launched : 2016
JSM Brain Science
ISSN : 2573-1289
Launched : 2016
JSM Biomarkers
ISSN : 2578-3815
Launched : 2014
JSM Biology
ISSN : 2475-9392
Launched : 2016
Archives of Stem Cell and Research
ISSN : 2578-3580
Launched : 2014
Annals of Clinical and Medical Microbiology
ISSN : 2578-3629
Launched : 2014
JSM Pediatric Surgery
ISSN : 2578-3149
Launched : 2017
Journal of Memory Disorder and Rehabilitation
ISSN : 2578-319X
Launched : 2016
JSM Tropical Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2578-3165
Launched : 2016
JSM Head and Face Medicine
ISSN : 2578-3793
Launched : 2016
JSM Cardiothoracic Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1297
Launched : 2016
JSM Bone and Joint Diseases
ISSN : 2578-3351
Launched : 2017
JSM Bioavailability and Bioequivalence
ISSN : 2641-7812
Launched : 2017
JSM Atherosclerosis
ISSN : 2573-1270
Launched : 2016
Journal of Genitourinary Disorders
ISSN : 2641-7790
Launched : 2017
Journal of Fractures and Sprains
ISSN : 2578-3831
Launched : 2016
Journal of Autism and Epilepsy
ISSN : 2641-7774
Launched : 2016
Annals of Marine Biology and Research
ISSN : 2573-105X
Launched : 2014
JSM Health Education & Primary Health Care
ISSN : 2578-3777
Launched : 2016
JSM Communication Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3807
Launched : 2016
Annals of Musculoskeletal Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3599
Launched : 2016
Annals of Virology and Research
ISSN : 2573-1122
Launched : 2014
JSM Renal Medicine
ISSN : 2573-1637
Launched : 2016
Journal of Muscle Health
ISSN : 2578-3823
Launched : 2016
JSM Genetics and Genomics
ISSN : 2334-1823
Launched : 2013
JSM Anxiety and Depression
ISSN : 2475-9139
Launched : 2016
Clinical Journal of Heart Diseases
ISSN : 2641-7766
Launched : 2016
Annals of Medicinal Chemistry and Research
ISSN : 2378-9336
Launched : 2014
JSM Pain and Management
ISSN : 2578-3378
Launched : 2016
JSM Women's Health
ISSN : 2578-3696
Launched : 2016
Clinical Research in HIV or AIDS
ISSN : 2374-0094
Launched : 2013
Journal of Endocrinology, Diabetes and Obesity
ISSN : 2333-6692
Launched : 2013
Journal of Substance Abuse and Alcoholism
ISSN : 2373-9363
Launched : 2013
JSM Neurosurgery and Spine
ISSN : 2373-9479
Launched : 2013
Journal of Liver and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2379-0830
Launched : 2014
Journal of Drug Design and Research
ISSN : 2379-089X
Launched : 2014
JSM Clinical Oncology and Research
ISSN : 2373-938X
Launched : 2013
JSM Bioinformatics, Genomics and Proteomics
ISSN : 2576-1102
Launched : 2014
JSM Chemistry
ISSN : 2334-1831
Launched : 2013
Journal of Trauma and Care
ISSN : 2573-1246
Launched : 2014
JSM Surgical Oncology and Research
ISSN : 2578-3688
Launched : 2016
Annals of Food Processing and Preservation
ISSN : 2573-1033
Launched : 2016
Journal of Radiology and Radiation Therapy
ISSN : 2333-7095
Launched : 2013
JSM Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation
ISSN : 2578-3572
Launched : 2016
Annals of Clinical Pathology
ISSN : 2373-9282
Launched : 2013
Annals of Cardiovascular Diseases
ISSN : 2641-7731
Launched : 2016
Journal of Behavior
ISSN : 2576-0076
Launched : 2016
Annals of Clinical and Experimental Metabolism
ISSN : 2572-2492
Launched : 2016
Clinical Research in Infectious Diseases
ISSN : 2379-0636
Launched : 2013
JSM Microbiology
ISSN : 2333-6455
Launched : 2013
Journal of Urology and Research
ISSN : 2379-951X
Launched : 2014
Journal of Family Medicine and Community Health
ISSN : 2379-0547
Launched : 2013
Annals of Pregnancy and Care
ISSN : 2578-336X
Launched : 2017
JSM Cell and Developmental Biology
ISSN : 2379-061X
Launched : 2013
Annals of Aquaculture and Research
ISSN : 2379-0881
Launched : 2014
Clinical Research in Pulmonology
ISSN : 2333-6625
Launched : 2013
Journal of Immunology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2333-6714
Launched : 2013
JSM Biochemistry and Molecular Biology
ISSN : 2333-7109
Launched : 2013
Annals of Breast Cancer Research
ISSN : 2641-7685
Launched : 2016
Annals of Gerontology and Geriatric Research
ISSN : 2378-9409
Launched : 2014
Journal of Sleep Medicine and Disorders
ISSN : 2379-0822
Launched : 2014
JSM Burns and Trauma
ISSN : 2475-9406
Launched : 2016
Chemical Engineering and Process Techniques
ISSN : 2333-6633
Launched : 2013
Annals of Clinical Cytology and Pathology
ISSN : 2475-9430
Launched : 2014
JSM Allergy and Asthma
ISSN : 2573-1254
Launched : 2016
Journal of Neurological Disorders and Stroke
ISSN : 2334-2307
Launched : 2013
Annals of Sports Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2379-0571
Launched : 2014
JSM Sexual Medicine
ISSN : 2578-3718
Launched : 2016
Annals of Vascular Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2378-9344
Launched : 2014
JSM Biotechnology and Biomedical Engineering
ISSN : 2333-7117
Launched : 2013
Journal of Hematology and Transfusion
ISSN : 2333-6684
Launched : 2013
JSM Environmental Science and Ecology
ISSN : 2333-7141
Launched : 2013
Journal of Cardiology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2333-6676
Launched : 2013
JSM Nanotechnology and Nanomedicine
ISSN : 2334-1815
Launched : 2013
Journal of Ear, Nose and Throat Disorders
ISSN : 2475-9473
Launched : 2016
JSM Ophthalmology
ISSN : 2333-6447
Launched : 2013
Journal of Pharmacology and Clinical Toxicology
ISSN : 2333-7079
Launched : 2013
Annals of Psychiatry and Mental Health
ISSN : 2374-0124
Launched : 2013
Medical Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology
ISSN : 2333-6439
Launched : 2013
Annals of Pediatrics and Child Health
ISSN : 2373-9312
Launched : 2013
JSM Clinical Pharmaceutics
ISSN : 2379-9498
Launched : 2014
JSM Foot and Ankle
ISSN : 2475-9112
Launched : 2016
JSM Alzheimer's Disease and Related Dementia
ISSN : 2378-9565
Launched : 2014
Journal of Addiction Medicine and Therapy
ISSN : 2333-665X
Launched : 2013
Journal of Veterinary Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2378-931X
Launched : 2013
Annals of Public Health and Research
ISSN : 2378-9328
Launched : 2014
Annals of Orthopedics and Rheumatology
ISSN : 2373-9290
Launched : 2013
Journal of Clinical Nephrology and Research
ISSN : 2379-0652
Launched : 2014
Annals of Community Medicine and Practice
ISSN : 2475-9465
Launched : 2014
Annals of Biometrics and Biostatistics
ISSN : 2374-0116
Launched : 2013
JSM Clinical Case Reports
ISSN : 2373-9819
Launched : 2013
Journal of Cancer Biology and Research
ISSN : 2373-9436
Launched : 2013
Journal of Surgery and Transplantation Science
ISSN : 2379-0911
Launched : 2013
Journal of Dermatology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2373-9371
Launched : 2013
JSM Gastroenterology and Hepatology
ISSN : 2373-9487
Launched : 2013
Annals of Nursing and Practice
ISSN : 2379-9501
Launched : 2014
JSM Dentistry
ISSN : 2333-7133
Launched : 2013
Author Information X