Loading

Annals of Forensic Research and Analysis

The Influence of Expectation on Verbal Memory Performance in Ecstasy Users

Research Article | Open Access | Volume 4 | Issue 1

  • 1. Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Cologne, Germany
+ Show More - Show Less
Corresponding Authors
Joerg Daumann University Hospital of Cologne, Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Kerpener Str. 12, 50924 Cologne, Germany, Tel: 00492214784001; Fax: 00492214783738
Abstract

Research on potential long-term consequences of ecstasy use suggests alterations in psychological well-being and cognitive performance. Due to methodological difficulties, it is still unclear whether this effect may be attributed to ecstasy itself or to other confounding factors, such as negative expectations. This study is aimed at discerning the effect of identifying with a negative stereotype upon performance on a battery of cognitive tests. Ecstasy users with self-reported decline were categorized by their subjective deterioration, attributed either to ecstasy use or to other reasons. Subjects were compared by their performance on frontal/executive functioning, episodic memory and information and processing speed. No effect has been observed for frontal/executive functioning and information and processing speed. However, the groups differed on total acquisition and recognition performance of the Auditiv Verbale Lerntest (AVLT). Verbal memory tasks seem to be susceptible to worries and fears generated by negative expectations in ecstasy users. This aspect should be taken into account while measuring cognitive decline in ecstasy users.

Keywords

N-Methyl-3,4-methylenedioxyamphetamine (MDMA); Ecstasy; Cognition; Expectation; Verbal memory.

CITATION

Wagner D, Hohnen M, Koester P, Gouzoulis-Mayfrank E, Daumann J (2017) The Influence of Expectation on Verbal Memory Performance in Ecstasy Users. Ann Forensic Res Anal 4(1): 1037.

ABBREVIATIONS

MDMA: N-Methyl-3, 4-Methylenedioxyamphetamine; SD: Standard Deviation; T1: Follow-Up Measurement 1; T2: Follow Up Measurement 2; AVLT: Auditiv-Verbale Lerntest (A German Version Of The Rey Verbal Learning Test); LGT: Lern- Und Gedächnistest (Figural Visual Recognition Test); TRAIL: Trail Making Test; STROOP: Stroop Test; DS Test: Digit Symbol Test

INTRODUCTION

The main psychoactive ingredient of the recreational drug “ecstasy” is MDMA (N-Methyl-3,4-methylenedioxyamphetamine). It is an amphetamine-based stimulant, which induces an acute increase in levels of the neurotransmitters serotonin and dopamine. Since the long-term effects regarding the abuse of said substance are not fully understood, a growing number of studies has emerged as a consequence. Repeated administration of MDMA to non-human animals results in neurotoxic degeneration of serotonergic axon terminals followed by a decrease of 5-HT concentration in brain tissue and this effect persists even after several years [1]. In contrast to animal studies, where structural brain changes have been observed, a number of studies with human subjects associate the regular use of MDMA with an increase in psychopathological symptoms such as anxiety, depression and psychosis as well as with a decrease in cognitive performance, apparent in executive dysfunction, impulse control disturbance and memory decline [2-4].

In addition to structural brain changes and psychopathological symptoms linked to drug abuse, there might be psychological mechanisms working as an intermediary effect on actual functioning. One such psychological mechanism is the placebo effect. People’s expectations concerning the effectiveness of their medication and treatments have a great impact on their actual usefulness [5]. Expectations about one’s own cognitive functioning might also influence actual cognitive performance, as has been observed in patients treated for depression. In a different study, negative expectation of subjects’ functioning was associated with lower cognitive performance on neuropsychological tests [4,6]. In addition, the awareness of a negative stereotype about a social group in a particular domain produces suboptimal performance by members of this group [7]. The influence of stereotype threat on cognitive performance has been extensively investigated in the past with respect to several stereotypes (e.g., women and math performance). It has been argued that task-related fears might interfere with working memory capacity. Its effective utilization is central for successful performance on complex cognitive tasks, and failures in working memory have been suggested as responsible for performance decrements. In these studies, participants were asked to solve math problems while keeping a set of words in memory. Later recollection of the words was used as an index of working memory capacity. These studies suggest that stereotype threat can disrupt the ability to maintain proper focus on task-relevant information [8].

The same might be true for ecstasy users. As a result of the wide media coverage of studies purporting to show cognitive deficits in ecstasy users, people belonging to this group might also be affected by stereotype threat. The effect of stereotype threat in ecstasy users has been investigated by comparing users and non-users with respect to working memory capacity, executive functioning and verbal memory capacity after they had been exposed to priming. The priming procedure consisted of exposing the subjects to information about the long-term effects of ecstasy, which either stated that ecstasy does or does not cause memory loss. Users who were informed that MDMA has no detrimental effects on memory performed better on the delayed portion of the prose task of the Rivermead Behavioural Memory Test battery than users having been negatively primed and even non-users [9]. These results suggest that, expectations due to a negative stereotype may have a greater influence on actual verbal memory test performance than the consumption of MDMA itself. The authors suggest that ecstasy users believe what they are told about the detrimental effects of ecstasy use on memory, but do not experience these problems themselves. However, in this study cognitive performance was directly affected by priming. It still remains unclear whether general subjective feelings of decline in physical and psychological well-being attributed to drug consumption have an effect on performance outcomes. Psychological mechanisms like self-evaluative threats might have a direct impact on tests intended to measure cognitive decline in drug users. This would be an important aspect to take into account for further meaningful research in the field of long-term effects of MDMA.

Thus the aim of this study was to assess whether such psychological mechanisms might play a role in mediating the cognitive performance of MDMA users; this was done by looking at the influence of their subjective feelings of cognitive and psychological decline on cognitive performance, especially in people attributing their deterioration to MDMA consumption.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Participants

The current study is based on data assembled with the help of 96 subjects at baseline (t0 ) [age range at t0 : 18-35 years; mean: 23.42 years; standard deviation (SD): 4.76] in a project with two follow-up measurements (t1 and t2 ).These follow-up measurements occurred one year and two years after the baseline measurement, respectively. Subjects were comprised of new ecstasy users with first but limited experience at t0 and no current or previous history of neurological or psychiatric disorders (Axes I and II according to DSM-IV criteria). Another exclusion criterion was the ingestion of any other illicit psychotropic substances on more than five occasions before the day of the first examination. This study was part of a bigger longitudinal project investigating the potential long-term effects of consumption in novice MDMA users. For more detailed information, the researchers kindly refer the reader to the publications of the initial studies [4,10-15].

Materials

The following questionnaires and tests were used to assemble the data that will be incorporated into the present study: First, a structured interview concerning the use of illicit psychotropic substances, gathering information such as the age of first use, number of days since the last use, estimated cumulative life time dose (relevant for the present study) as well as the highest dose ever used and the duration of regular use measured in months. Second, a questionnaire containing questions about the subjective evaluation of a possible deterioration with regard to the participants’ physical and psychological well-being. Furthermore, this questionnaire assessed the supposed reasons (e.g., MDMA consumption) participants ascribed to their self perceived decline on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = not likely, 5 = very likely). Third, a comprehensive neuropsychological test battery assessing the function domain episodic memory, frontal executive functioning and attention and information processing speed. The episodic memory was measured using the Auditiv verbale Lerntest (AVLT) and Lern- und Gedächnistest (LGT). The AVLT is a German version of the Rey Verbal Learning test (RAVLT), and was used in order to assess verbal declarative memory performance by means of the variables, immediate recall, total acquisition performance, delayed recall after interference, loss after interference and recognition after a delay of 30 minutes. A subtest of the LGT was used to measure immediate and figural visual recognition. This instrument is a classical paired association-learning task containing 20 different logos, each composed of a central figure and surrounded by a frame. During immediate and delayed assessment the subjects were asked to find the correct frame associated with the central figures. Frontal executive functioning was assessed by means of the digit span test, the Stroop task and the Trail-Making Test. The digit span test is a component of the German version of the Wechsler Intelligence Test (WAIS), the Hamburger-Wechsler Intelligenztestfür Erwachsene (HAWIE-R), which is used for assessing working memory. The subjects listen to a sequence of digits and are asked to recall them immediately in reverse order. The cognitive interference was assessed with a German pencil-and-paper version of the Stroop task. The participants are instructed under different conditions to either read out the names of the colors or the name of the ink in which the name of the colors is presented. The recording times as well as the corrected and uncorrected errors represent relevant criteria. The Trail-Making Test measures mental flexibility and consists of two different parts. In the first part the subjects have to connect, in ascending order, numbers in circles (1-25) on a sheet of paper as quickly as possible. In the second part the subjects are presented with numbers (1-13) and additional letters (A-L), and have to connect the numbers and letters alternately in correct ascending order (i.e., 1-A-2-B-3-C). The response time is recorded during each measurement. Attention and information processing speed are measured by the Digit Symbol Test, the Stroop Task and the Trail-Making Test. The Digit Symbol Test, which is also part of the WAIS, consists of nine digit-symbol pairs. After having acquired these pairs correctly, the subjects have 90 seconds to transform a list of 93 digits as quickly as possible into the associated symbols. The selection of tests is based on the results of previous cross sectional and longitudinal studies of ecstasy users [4].

Procedure

Preceding the actual experiment, written informed consent was given to all participants. It provided information about the nature of the experiment, i.e. possible long-term effects of ecstasy use. Further it provided information of neurotoxic findings in animal studies by very high doses of MDMA. All of the aforementioned questionnaires, structured interviews and tests were administered at every time measurement point during the longitudinal study. In the present study, participants were subdivided into groups according to their subjective deterioration attributed to MDMA on questionnaire number 2: Subjects with high ratings (≥ 3) on MDMA as reason for their deterioration and low ratings (< 3) on the remaining possible reasons for their deterioration were classified in one group (MDMA), while subjects with low ratings on MDMA and high scores on any of the other reasons were placed in the second group (non-MDMA). Subjects from each group were paired, when possible, with a match-partner according to their respective amounts of ecstasy consumption, in order to correct for consumption. The resulting variable served as a group factor. When no fitting match-partner could be found, the subject was not included in the analysis. Thus, of the initial 96 subjects by t0 , 22 remained for the t1 assessment and 14 subjects for the t2 follow-up.

Statistical analyses

At first, change scores of the results of the participants´ neuropsychological tests of participants included in the analysis were computed by subtracting the scores of t1 and t2 from t0 taking into account performance at baseline level, yielding variables T1 and T2 . Multivariate analyses of variance (MANOVA) were conducted with group factor (MDMA vs. non-MDMA) and change scores per function domain (episodic memory, frontal executive functioning and information and processing speed) and measurement point (T1 and T2 ) as dependent variables. All analyses were performed with IBM SPSS statistical software program version 21 (Chicago, IL, USA)

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION

Group characteristics

After matching members of each group based on the amount of consumption, 22 participants remained for the group T1 and 14 participants for the group of T2 . For each group, gender distribution, mean age, years of education, and mean cumulative use of MDMA as well as the corresponding standard deviations are given in Table (1).

Table 1: Group characteristics.

 

Female/Male

Age

Years of education

Cumulative MDMA use*

MDMA

1/6

24.14 (± 2.27)

13.43 (± 1.81)

11.86 (± 21.40)

Non-MDMA

3/4

23.14 (±5.87)

15.29 (± 1.38)

11.86 (± 21.40)

*at t2

Abbreviations: MDMA: N-Methyl-3, 4-methylenedioxyamphetamine

The groups did not differ significantly with respect to any of the variables.

Performance effects

The MANOVA addressing episodic memory for T1 revealed no significant main effect of group (MDMA versus non-MDMA) (F(9,3) = 1.00, p = .49, chi2 = .45). The analyses for T2 revealed a significant main effect of group (F(9,3) = 22.83, p = .01, chi2 = .99). The corresponding tests of between-subject effects revealed significant effects of total acquisition (F(1,11) = 5.40, p = .04, chi2 = .33) and recognition performance (F(1,11) = 7.28, p = .02, chi2 = .40) of the verbal declarative memory performance test but not for any other variable. Mean test scores for both groups as well as significant levels of the corresponding tests of between-subject effects are given in Table (2).

Table 2: Mean test change scores - episodic memory.

 

MDMA

non-MDMA

p

MEAN (SD)

MEAN (SD)

 

T1

T2

T1

T2

T1

T2

AVLT A

0.09 (3.17)

-7.57 (20.70)

-1.18 (3.12)

-1.29 (1.89)

.39

.48

AVLT B

-1.55 (3.53)

-0.57 (2.15)

1.00 (2.53)

2.00 (1.53)

.07

.04*

AVLT C

-0.36 (3.59)

0.71 (2.23)

-0.55 (3.30)

0.86 (2.12)

.61

.90

AVLT D

-1.55 (2.50)

-1.29 (2.87)

0.36 (1.96)

-0.14 (1.21)

.53

.08

AVLT E

0.45 (1.97)

0.57 (1.51)

-4.5 (1.97)

1.57 (2.37)

.20

.02*

AVLT F

0.91 (1.87)

0.14 (0.90)

0.9 (1.87)

-0.43 (1,90)

.49

.19

LGT A

1.18 (2.68)

-1.00 (6.11)

1.55 (5.43)

-2.43 (6.50)

.62

.86

LGT B

0.55 (4.89)

- 3.14 (6.01)

-0.36 (5.94)

-2.79 (6.18)

.98

.40

*sig

Abbreviations: MDMA: N-Methyl-3, 4-methylenedioxyamphetamine; SD: standard deviation; T1: follow-up measurement 1; T2: follow-up measurement 2; AVLT: Auditiv-verbale Lerntest (a German version of the Rey Verbal Learning test), A: immediate recall, B: total acquisition, C: recall after interference, D: loss after interference, E: recognition performance, F: repetitions required for learning; LGT: Lern- und Gedächnistest (figural visual recognition test)

Additionally, for each group, means and standard deviations of both variables of the AVLT are given in Figure (1).

Figure 1: Means and standard deviations of significant AVLT change scores (T2 ).

MDMA: subjects with high ratings on ecstasy as reason for their deterioration and low ratings on the remaining reasons, non-MDMA: subjects with low ratings on ecstasy and high scores on any of the other reasons.

The MANOVA addressing attention and information processing speed for T1 revealed no significant main effect of group (F(4,16) = 1.75, p = .19 , chi2 = .30). The same was true for T2 (F(4,8) = 1.88, p = .20, chi2 = .49). Mean test scores for both groups as well as significant levels of the corresponding tests of between subject effects are given in Table (3).

Table 3: Mean test change scores - attention and information processing speed.

 

MDMA

non-MDMA

p

MEAN (SD)

MEAN (SD)

 

T1

T2

T1

T2

T1

T2

Trail A

4.94 (7.10)

3.50 (5.52)

7.41 (8.91)

3.60 (10.38)

.33

0.16

Stroop A

9.25 (9.65)

7.39 (4.95)

1.98 (5.25)

.31 (4.20)

.07

.01*

Stroop B

7.88 (11.49)

1.58 (12.66)

5.78 (8.51)

5.53 (6.82)

.74

.28

DS Test

68.18 (11.47)

-13.57 (18.20) 

67.91 (15.35) 

7.57 (14.06) 

.87 

.42 

*sig

Abbreviations: MDMA: N-Methyl-3, 4-methylenedioxyamphetamine; SD: standard deviation; T1: follow-up measurement 1; T2: follow-up measurement 2; TRAIL: Trail-Making Test; STROOP: Stroop Test; DS Test: Digit Symbol Test

The MANOVA addressing frontal executive functioning for T1 revealed no significant main effect of group (F(3,17) = 1.16, p = .36, chi2 = .17). The same was true for T2 (F(3,9) = 2.29, p = .15, chi2 = .43). Mean test scores for both groups as well as significant levels of the corresponding tests of between-subject effects are given in Table (4).

Table 4: Mean test change scores - frontal executive functioning.

 

MDMA

non-MDMA

p

MEAN (SD)

MEAN (SD)

 

T1

T2

T1

T2

T1

T2

Stroop C

12.57 (15.16)

10.10 (21.60)

9.21 (13.14)

14.83 (22.12)

.51

.53

DS back

-0.45 (3.62)

-0.43 (2.76)

2.00 (2.41)

0.14 (3.02)

.09

.72

Trail B

18.50 (15.88)

 26.49 (24.33)

15.91 (24.76)

7.97 (34.92)

.88

.20

Abbreviations: MDMA: N-Methyl-3, 4-methylenedioxyamphetamine; SD: standard deviation; T1: follow-up measurement 1; T2: follow-up measurement 2; STROOP: Stroop Test; DS Test: Digit Symbol Test; TRAIL: Trail-Making Test

DISCUSSION

The aim of this study was to assess whether psychological mechanisms like self-evaluative threat might play a role in mediating the cognitive performance of ecstasy users. This was done by looking at the influence of their subjective feelings of cognitive and psychological decline on cognitive performance, especially in people attributing their deterioration to MDMA consumption. Therefore, subjects were categorized by their subjective deterioration attributed either to MDMA or to other reasons. After matching all participants according to their respective amounts of ecstasy consumption, 22 of the initial 96 subjects remained for the first follow-up assessment and 14 subjects for the second follow-up. Both groups were compared by their performance on cognitive tests originally intended to measure cognitive performance in MDMA users across the domains, episodic memory, information and processing speed, as well as frontal executive functioning. The groups did not differ with respect to the domains, frontal executive functioning and information and processing speed. However, significant group differences were found at the second measurement point for the domain of episodic memory especially regarding total acquisition and recognition performance of the verbal declarative memory performance test, but not for any other variable. Subjects not ascribing their subjective decline to MDMA performed better on these two tests compared to subjects ascribing their subjective decline to the use of the drug. Thus, negative effects of self-evaluation were not confirmed either for information and processing speed or for frontal executive functioning, but seemed to affect episodic memory performance of the participants ascribing their decline to MDMA consumption.

Effects of self-evaluative threat (e.g., stereotype threat) on memory performance have been described previously [8]. Particular verbal memory seems to be susceptible to task-related anxiety and worries, which interfere with phonological aspects of working memory and is therefore susceptible to negative performance expectations [7]. This is in line with the study where priming of a stereotype (MDMA causes memory loss vs. MDMA causes no memory loss) affected verbal memory performance of ecstasy users but not working memory and executive functioning performance [9]. The current findings support this theory and provide an alternative explanation for memory deficits observed in a number of studies concerning the long-term effects of MDMA. The present study is based on a longitudinal study which showed no difference on the AVLT test between ecstasy users and non-users [4]. In the present study, participants showed differences on the AVLT test according to their self-attributed reasons for deterioration. While some studies have found decreases in memory performance in ecstasy users, other studies could not confirm these results [16]. Cognitively more complex memory tasks (e.g., the California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT), which is similar to the AVLT) seem to increase the likelihood of potential ecstasy-related behavioral deficits in long-term verbal memory [17]. On the other hand, highly complex tasks require more working memory capacity and are therefore more susceptible to task-irrelevant aspects like performance worries [8]. Inconsistencies in results may arise because performance expectations have a greater influence on actual verbal memory test performance than the impact of MDMA itself. Therefore, studies on performance decrements of ecstasy users relative to non-users should be interpreted carefully.

The relation between ecstasy use and memory deficits is widely reported in the media, creating a stereotype with respect to ecstasy users. In the study by [9], ecstasy users believed what they were told about detrimental effects of ecstasy use on memory (priming), but did not experience these problems themselves. The awareness of a negative stereotype might not be enough to evoke negative performance expectations. Therefore, the present study compared subjects according to their subjective self-evaluations of experiencing detriments and according to their reasons for deterioration. Self-reported problems attributed to ecstasy seem to reflect actual decline in well-being. As has been shown, subjects who perceived themselves as problematic ecstasy users indeed showed evidence of a range of symptoms. The same was true for subjects defining themselves as non-problematic, who showed no symptoms [18]. In the present study, both groups experienced cognitive and psychological decline and therefore might both suffer from fears. Nevertheless, participants attributing their decline to reasons other than MDMA showed better verbal memory performances on the AVLT. Since all participants were aware, due to informed consent, that the study was aimed at testing potential long-term effects of drug consumption, subjects attributing their deterioration to MDMA consumption may suffer from worries concerning their performance. On the other hand, subjects not attributing their deteriorations to MDMA use may not suffer from fears and subsequently perform better. This modulation effect has been observed in cannabis users. While men performed worse on a memory task under negative stereotype priming, female users were not affected and performed even better than female users who were not primed at all. The authors suggest that this surprising gender effect originates in differences in identification with the stereotype invented by priming procedure. Since men seem to identify more with the stereotype of a typical cannabis user, they may be more susceptible to the negative impact of stereotype threat. Women not identifying with the stereotype of a cannabis-user might experience stereotype lift and consequently improve their performance. Therefore stereotype threat might even work in opposite ways: the awareness of a negative stereotype, while not identifying with it, may lead the subject to extra task involvement and thus to higher performance [16]. Furthermore, a fMRI study showed altered brain activation during semantic processing in ecstasy users relative to non-users, but they performed equivalently on cognitive performance tests. The authors concluded that ecstasy users might rely on compensatory mechanisms establishing the same cognitive performance as non-users [19]. In this case, drug-related decrements due to MDMA consumption would not be reflected in cognitive test performance. Thus, differences in cognitive performance may account for more variability within the group of ecstasy users than between ecstasy users and non users.

As a consequence, it is suggested that future research concerning potential long-term effects of MDMA on cognitive performance should strive to minimize the possible influence of negative expectations on test scores. How could this be accomplished? First of all, subjects should preferably remain naïve to the purpose of the study in which they are participating. However, concerning research on illicit substances, this may be impossible to achieve, since participants recruited for their experience with certain drugs will probably realize the connection between their substance use and the types of cognitive performance tests conducted. In this case, a number of interventions aimed at reducing stereotype threat might prove more useful. For example, the negative effects of stereotype threat on performance in a math task could be completely reversed by having subjects perform a brief mindfulness exercise. According to the authors, mindfulness can be utilized to alleviate working memory load, thereby freeing up the very resource drained by anxiety resulting from stereotype threat [20]. Another study suggests that priming positive feelings about him/ herself might be equally beneficial with respect to decreasing the impact of stereotype threat. Women who were primed to feel powerful did not show reductions in working memory capacity or math performance in response to threatening stereotypes [21]. Hence, implementing a short mindfulness exercise or priming a positive self-image before taking cognitive tests may increase the accuracy of results in studies on MDMA and cognitive performance.

Although the results of the current study partly support previous findings concerning the impact of negative expectation on verbal memory performance, it should be noted that the number of participants in this study was quite small. Of the original 96 subjects, only 22 participants remained for the first follow-up and 14 for the second follow-up measurement. The small number of participants resulted in a weak power of the study. In order to obtain more meaningful results, it is therefore advisable to conduct a study with more subjects. Furthermore, this study had no experimental design; subjects were selected by their subjective feelings of physical and psychological decline, and groups were formed based on their ratings on proposed reasons. Therefore, the present findings should be interpreted carefully, and causal relationships between verbal memory performance and negative expectations should be pondered critically. Another limitation of the present study is that the drug history of the participants was based on their self-reported consumption behavior. The reliability of such statements should be considered carefully.

CONCLUSION

Despite these methodological problems, there is growing evidence that observed memory deficits in MDMA research cannot be attributed with certainty to direct effects of the drug alone. In conclusion, researchers are advised to take precautionary steps to alleviate the influence of negative expectations on cognitive test results and to be cautious about presenting their findings to the media. Otherwise, the public stereotype about the detrimental effects of ecstasy use might be aggravated, thus further interfering with effective research on the potential long term effects of MDMA.

CONFLICT OF INTEREST

This work was supported by a grant to E. Gouzoulis-Mayfrank and J. Daumann from the German Research Foundation (Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft DFG, Go 717/6-1/2). Furthermore, the authors declare that, except for income received from their primary employer, no financial support or compensation has been received from any individual or corporate entity over the past 36 months for research or professional service, and there are no personal financial holdings that could be perceived as constituting a potential conflict of interest. This pertains to all the authors of the study, their spouses or partners and their children (aged under 18).

REFERENCES

1. Gouzoulis-Mayfrank E, Daumann J. Neurotoxicity of drugs of abuse--the case of methylenedioxyamphetamines (MDMA, ecstasy), and amphetamines. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2009; 11: 305-317.

2. Lieb R, Schuetz CG, Pfister H, von Sydow K, Wittchen HU. Mental disorders in ecstasy users: a prospective-longitudinal investigation. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2002; 68: 195-207.

3. Medina KL, Shear PK. Anxiety, depression, and behavioral symptoms of executive dysfunction in ecstasy users: Contributions of polydrug use. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2007; 87: 303-311.

4. Wagner D, Becker B, Koester P, Gouzoulis-Mayfrank E, Daumann J. A prospective study of learning, memory, and executive function in new MDMA users. Addiction. 2013; 108: 136-145.

5. Schedlowski M, Rief W, Enck P. Placebo- und Noceboeffekte: Die große Herausforderung für die Verhaltenswissenschaften in der MedizinPlacebo- and nocebo effects: The great challenge for the behavioral sciences in medicine. Zeitschrift für Medizinische Psychologie. 2010; 19: 107-109.

6. Mitsikostas DD, Mantonakis L, Chalarakis N. Nocebo in clinical trials for depression: A meta-analysis. Psychiatry Res. 2014; 215: 82-86.

7. Beilock SL, Rydell RJ, McConnell AR. Stereotype threat and working memory: Mechanisms, alleviation, and spillover. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2007; 136: 256-276.

8. Schmader T, Johns M. Converging Evidence That Stereotype Threat Reduces Working Memory Capacity. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2003; 85: 440-452.

9. Cole J, Michailidou K, Jerome L, Sumnall H. The effects of stereotype threat on cognitive function in ecstasy users. J Psychopharmacol. 2006; 20: 518-525.

10. Becker B, Wagner D, Koester P, Bender K, Kabbasch C, Gouzoulis Mayfrank E, et al. Memory-related hippocampal functioning in ecstasy and amphetamine users: A prospective fMRI study. Psychopharmacology. 2013; 225: 923-934.

11. Daumann J, Koester P, Becker B, Wagner D, Imperati D, Gouzoulis Mayfrank E, et al. Medial prefrontal gray matter volume reductions in users of amphetamine-type stimulants revealed by combined tract based spatial statistics and voxel-based morphometry. Neuroimage. 2011; 54: 794-801.

12. Koester P, Tittgemeyer M, Wagner D, Becker B, Gouzoulis-Mayfrank E, Daumann J. Cortical thinning in amphetamine-type stimulant users. Neuroscience. 2012; 221: 182-192.

13. Koester P, Volz KG, Tittgemeyer M, Wagner D, Becker B, Gouzoulis Mayfrank E, et al. Decision-making in Polydrug Amphetamine-type Stimulant Users: an fMRI Study. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2013; 38: 1377-1386.

14. Wagner D, Becker B, Gouzoulis-Mayfrank E, Daumann J. Interactions between specific parameters of cannabis use and verbal memory. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2010; 34: 871-876.

15. Wagner D, Koester P, Becker B, Gouzoulis-Mayfrank E, Hellmich M, Daumann J. A longitudinal study of self-reported psychopathology in early ecstasy and amphetamine users. Psychopharmacology. 2015; 232: 897-905.

16. Looby A, Earleywine M. Gender moderates the impact of stereotype threat on cognitive function in cannabis users. Addict Behav. 2010; 35: 834-839.

17. Brown J, McKone E, Ward J. Deficits of long-term memory in ecstasy users are related to cognitive complexity of the task. Psychopharmacology. 2010; 209: 51-67. 

18. Soar K, Turner JJD, Parrott AC. Problematic versus non-problematic ecstasy/MDMA use: the influence of drug usage patterns and pre existing psychiatric factors. J Psychopharmacol. 2006; 20: 417-424.

19. Watkins TJ, Raj V, Lee J, Dietrich MS, Cao AZ, Blackford JU, et al. Human ecstasy (MDMA) polydrug users have altered brain activation during semantic processing. Psychopharmacology. 2013; 227: 41-54. Cite this article

20. Weger UW, Hooper N, Meier BP, Hopthrow T. Mindful maths: Reducing the impact of stereotype threat through a mindfulness exercise. Conscious Cogn. 2012; 21: 471-475.

21. Van Loo KJ, Rydell RJ. On the experience of feeling powerful: Perceived power moderates the effect of stereotype threat on women’s math performance. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2013; 39: 387-400.

Wagner D, Hohnen M, Koester P, Gouzoulis-Mayfrank E, Daumann J (2017) The Influence of Expectation on Verbal Memory Performance in Ecstasy Users. Ann Forensic Res Anal 4(1): 1037.

Received : 17 Jan 2017
Accepted : 08 Feb 2017
Published : 09 Feb 2017
Journals
Annals of Otolaryngology and Rhinology
ISSN : 2379-948X
Launched : 2014
JSM Schizophrenia
Launched : 2016
Journal of Nausea
Launched : 2020
JSM Internal Medicine
Launched : 2016
JSM Hepatitis
Launched : 2016
JSM Oro Facial Surgeries
ISSN : 2578-3211
Launched : 2016
Journal of Human Nutrition and Food Science
ISSN : 2333-6706
Launched : 2013
JSM Regenerative Medicine and Bioengineering
ISSN : 2379-0490
Launched : 2013
JSM Spine
ISSN : 2578-3181
Launched : 2016
Archives of Palliative Care
ISSN : 2573-1165
Launched : 2016
JSM Nutritional Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3203
Launched : 2017
Annals of Neurodegenerative Disorders
ISSN : 2476-2032
Launched : 2016
Journal of Fever
ISSN : 2641-7782
Launched : 2017
JSM Bone Marrow Research
ISSN : 2578-3351
Launched : 2016
JSM Mathematics and Statistics
ISSN : 2578-3173
Launched : 2014
Journal of Autoimmunity and Research
ISSN : 2573-1173
Launched : 2014
JSM Arthritis
ISSN : 2475-9155
Launched : 2016
JSM Head and Neck Cancer-Cases and Reviews
ISSN : 2573-1610
Launched : 2016
JSM General Surgery Cases and Images
ISSN : 2573-1564
Launched : 2016
JSM Anatomy and Physiology
ISSN : 2573-1262
Launched : 2016
JSM Dental Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1548
Launched : 2016
Annals of Emergency Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1017
Launched : 2016
Annals of Mens Health and Wellness
ISSN : 2641-7707
Launched : 2017
Journal of Preventive Medicine and Health Care
ISSN : 2576-0084
Launched : 2018
Journal of Chronic Diseases and Management
ISSN : 2573-1300
Launched : 2016
Annals of Vaccines and Immunization
ISSN : 2378-9379
Launched : 2014
JSM Heart Surgery Cases and Images
ISSN : 2578-3157
Launched : 2016
Annals of Reproductive Medicine and Treatment
ISSN : 2573-1092
Launched : 2016
JSM Brain Science
ISSN : 2573-1289
Launched : 2016
JSM Biomarkers
ISSN : 2578-3815
Launched : 2014
JSM Biology
ISSN : 2475-9392
Launched : 2016
Archives of Stem Cell and Research
ISSN : 2578-3580
Launched : 2014
Annals of Clinical and Medical Microbiology
ISSN : 2578-3629
Launched : 2014
JSM Pediatric Surgery
ISSN : 2578-3149
Launched : 2017
Journal of Memory Disorder and Rehabilitation
ISSN : 2578-319X
Launched : 2016
JSM Tropical Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2578-3165
Launched : 2016
JSM Head and Face Medicine
ISSN : 2578-3793
Launched : 2016
JSM Cardiothoracic Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1297
Launched : 2016
JSM Bone and Joint Diseases
ISSN : 2578-3351
Launched : 2017
JSM Bioavailability and Bioequivalence
ISSN : 2641-7812
Launched : 2017
JSM Atherosclerosis
ISSN : 2573-1270
Launched : 2016
Journal of Genitourinary Disorders
ISSN : 2641-7790
Launched : 2017
Journal of Fractures and Sprains
ISSN : 2578-3831
Launched : 2016
Journal of Autism and Epilepsy
ISSN : 2641-7774
Launched : 2016
Annals of Marine Biology and Research
ISSN : 2573-105X
Launched : 2014
JSM Health Education & Primary Health Care
ISSN : 2578-3777
Launched : 2016
JSM Communication Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3807
Launched : 2016
Annals of Musculoskeletal Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3599
Launched : 2016
Annals of Virology and Research
ISSN : 2573-1122
Launched : 2014
JSM Renal Medicine
ISSN : 2573-1637
Launched : 2016
Journal of Muscle Health
ISSN : 2578-3823
Launched : 2016
JSM Genetics and Genomics
ISSN : 2334-1823
Launched : 2013
JSM Anxiety and Depression
ISSN : 2475-9139
Launched : 2016
Clinical Journal of Heart Diseases
ISSN : 2641-7766
Launched : 2016
Annals of Medicinal Chemistry and Research
ISSN : 2378-9336
Launched : 2014
JSM Pain and Management
ISSN : 2578-3378
Launched : 2016
JSM Women's Health
ISSN : 2578-3696
Launched : 2016
Clinical Research in HIV or AIDS
ISSN : 2374-0094
Launched : 2013
Journal of Endocrinology, Diabetes and Obesity
ISSN : 2333-6692
Launched : 2013
Journal of Substance Abuse and Alcoholism
ISSN : 2373-9363
Launched : 2013
JSM Neurosurgery and Spine
ISSN : 2373-9479
Launched : 2013
Journal of Liver and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2379-0830
Launched : 2014
Journal of Drug Design and Research
ISSN : 2379-089X
Launched : 2014
JSM Clinical Oncology and Research
ISSN : 2373-938X
Launched : 2013
JSM Bioinformatics, Genomics and Proteomics
ISSN : 2576-1102
Launched : 2014
JSM Chemistry
ISSN : 2334-1831
Launched : 2013
Journal of Trauma and Care
ISSN : 2573-1246
Launched : 2014
JSM Surgical Oncology and Research
ISSN : 2578-3688
Launched : 2016
Annals of Food Processing and Preservation
ISSN : 2573-1033
Launched : 2016
Journal of Radiology and Radiation Therapy
ISSN : 2333-7095
Launched : 2013
JSM Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation
ISSN : 2578-3572
Launched : 2016
Annals of Clinical Pathology
ISSN : 2373-9282
Launched : 2013
Annals of Cardiovascular Diseases
ISSN : 2641-7731
Launched : 2016
Journal of Behavior
ISSN : 2576-0076
Launched : 2016
Annals of Clinical and Experimental Metabolism
ISSN : 2572-2492
Launched : 2016
Clinical Research in Infectious Diseases
ISSN : 2379-0636
Launched : 2013
JSM Microbiology
ISSN : 2333-6455
Launched : 2013
Journal of Urology and Research
ISSN : 2379-951X
Launched : 2014
Journal of Family Medicine and Community Health
ISSN : 2379-0547
Launched : 2013
Annals of Pregnancy and Care
ISSN : 2578-336X
Launched : 2017
JSM Cell and Developmental Biology
ISSN : 2379-061X
Launched : 2013
Annals of Aquaculture and Research
ISSN : 2379-0881
Launched : 2014
Clinical Research in Pulmonology
ISSN : 2333-6625
Launched : 2013
Journal of Immunology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2333-6714
Launched : 2013
JSM Biochemistry and Molecular Biology
ISSN : 2333-7109
Launched : 2013
Annals of Breast Cancer Research
ISSN : 2641-7685
Launched : 2016
Annals of Gerontology and Geriatric Research
ISSN : 2378-9409
Launched : 2014
Journal of Sleep Medicine and Disorders
ISSN : 2379-0822
Launched : 2014
JSM Burns and Trauma
ISSN : 2475-9406
Launched : 2016
Chemical Engineering and Process Techniques
ISSN : 2333-6633
Launched : 2013
Annals of Clinical Cytology and Pathology
ISSN : 2475-9430
Launched : 2014
JSM Allergy and Asthma
ISSN : 2573-1254
Launched : 2016
Journal of Neurological Disorders and Stroke
ISSN : 2334-2307
Launched : 2013
Annals of Sports Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2379-0571
Launched : 2014
JSM Sexual Medicine
ISSN : 2578-3718
Launched : 2016
Annals of Vascular Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2378-9344
Launched : 2014
JSM Biotechnology and Biomedical Engineering
ISSN : 2333-7117
Launched : 2013
Journal of Hematology and Transfusion
ISSN : 2333-6684
Launched : 2013
JSM Environmental Science and Ecology
ISSN : 2333-7141
Launched : 2013
Journal of Cardiology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2333-6676
Launched : 2013
JSM Nanotechnology and Nanomedicine
ISSN : 2334-1815
Launched : 2013
Journal of Ear, Nose and Throat Disorders
ISSN : 2475-9473
Launched : 2016
JSM Ophthalmology
ISSN : 2333-6447
Launched : 2013
Journal of Pharmacology and Clinical Toxicology
ISSN : 2333-7079
Launched : 2013
Annals of Psychiatry and Mental Health
ISSN : 2374-0124
Launched : 2013
Medical Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology
ISSN : 2333-6439
Launched : 2013
Annals of Pediatrics and Child Health
ISSN : 2373-9312
Launched : 2013
JSM Clinical Pharmaceutics
ISSN : 2379-9498
Launched : 2014
JSM Foot and Ankle
ISSN : 2475-9112
Launched : 2016
JSM Alzheimer's Disease and Related Dementia
ISSN : 2378-9565
Launched : 2014
Journal of Addiction Medicine and Therapy
ISSN : 2333-665X
Launched : 2013
Journal of Veterinary Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2378-931X
Launched : 2013
Annals of Public Health and Research
ISSN : 2378-9328
Launched : 2014
Annals of Orthopedics and Rheumatology
ISSN : 2373-9290
Launched : 2013
Journal of Clinical Nephrology and Research
ISSN : 2379-0652
Launched : 2014
Annals of Community Medicine and Practice
ISSN : 2475-9465
Launched : 2014
Annals of Biometrics and Biostatistics
ISSN : 2374-0116
Launched : 2013
JSM Clinical Case Reports
ISSN : 2373-9819
Launched : 2013
Journal of Cancer Biology and Research
ISSN : 2373-9436
Launched : 2013
Journal of Surgery and Transplantation Science
ISSN : 2379-0911
Launched : 2013
Journal of Dermatology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2373-9371
Launched : 2013
JSM Gastroenterology and Hepatology
ISSN : 2373-9487
Launched : 2013
Annals of Nursing and Practice
ISSN : 2379-9501
Launched : 2014
JSM Dentistry
ISSN : 2333-7133
Launched : 2013
Author Information X