Loading

Annals of Pediatrics and Child Health

Association between Temperament and Nutrition Risk in Pre-School Children

Research Article | Open Access

  • 1. Pediatrics Outcomes Research Team, Division of Pediatric Medicine, The Hospital for Sick Children, Canada
  • 2. Institute of Health Policy, University of Toronto, Canada
  • 3. Li KaShing Knowledge Institute of St. Michael’s Hospital, Canada
  • 4. Department of Pediatrics, St. Michael’s Hospital, Canada
  • 5. Child Health Evaluative Sciences, Hospital for Sick Children Research Institute, Canada
  • 6. Department of Pediatrics, University of Toronto, Canada
  • 7. Human Development and Applied Psychology, University of Toronto, Canada
+ Show More - Show Less
Corresponding Authors
Patricia C. Parkin, Division of Paediatric Medicine, Department of Paediatrics, The Hospital for Sick Children, Room 109708, 10th Floor - Peter Gilgan Centre for Research and Learning, 686 Bay Street, Toronto, ON M5G 0A4 Canada, Tel: 416-813-7654 ext. 301544; Fax: 416 813-5663
ABSTRACT

Objective: To determine whether there is an association between parent reported child temperament and nutrition risk in preschool aged children.

Methods: This was a cross-sectional study. Healthy children aged between 3-5 years were recruited from community-based primary care practices. Temperament was assessed using the Children’s Behavior Questionnaire Very Short Form (CBQ-VSQ) which identifies three domains: Negative Affectivity, Effortful Control, and Extraversion/Surgency. Nutrition risk was assessed using the Nutrition Screening Tool for Every Pre-schooler Questionnaire (NutriSTEPTM). Adjusted logistic regression was used to measure the association between individual temperament domain and nutrition risk.

Result: 3468 children were included (mean age 3.75 years, 48 % female) in the study. The mean (SD) of Negative Affectivity, Effortful Control, and Extraversion/Surgency, were 3.79 (0.85), 5.46 (0.72) and 4.56 (0.79) respectively. About 14% (n=472) of the children had high nutrition risk. Children who scored high in Negative Affectivity and high in Effortful Control had an odds ratio of 1.47 (95% CI: 1.30, 1.66, p = <0.001) and 0.73 (95% CI: 0.64, 0.84, p = <0.001) respectively for high nutrition risk.

Conclusion: This study found children whose temperament is characterized by Negative Affectivity (discomfort, fear, anger/frustration, sadness and poor soothability) are more likely to be associated with high nutrition risk. These concepts can alert physicians to behavior patterns in children that may predispose them to poor nutrition and guide clinical decision making through laboratory testing for nutrition status, counseling and close monitoring.

CITATION

Abdullah K, Lebovic G, Birken CS, Maguire JL, Jenkins J, et al. (2015) Association between Temperament and Nutrition Risk in Pre-School Children. Ann Pediatr Child Health 3(9): 1088.

KEYWORDS

•    Negative affectivity
•    Effortful control
•    Surgency
•    Nutrition risk
•    Pre-school children

TARGET KIDS! COLLABORATION

Scientific Committee: Kawsari Abdullah, Laura N Anderson, Catherine S Birken, Cornelia M Borkhoff, Sarah Carsley, Yang Chen, Mikael Katz-Lavigne, KanthiKavikondala, Christine Koroshegyi, Grace Jieun Lee, Jonathon L Maguire, Dalah Mason, Jessica Omand, Patricia C Parkin, NavindraPersaud, Meta van den Heuvel, WeedaZabih, ImaanBayoumi, Christine Kowal and Peter Wong. Site Investigators: Jillian Baker, Tony Barozzino, Joey Bonifacio, Douglas Campbell, Sohail Cheema, Brian Chisamore, KaroonDanayan, Paul Das, Mary Beth Derocher, Anh Do, Michael Dorey, Sloane Freeman, Keewai Fung, Charlie Guiang, Curtis Handford, Hailey Hatch, Sheila Jacobson, Tara Kiran, Holly Knowles, Bruce Kwok, Sheila Lakhoo, Margarita Lam-Antoniades, Eddy Lau, Fok-Han Leung, Jennifer Loo, Sarah Mahmoud, Rosemary Moodie, Julia Morinis, Sharon Naymark, Patricia Neelands, James Owen, Michael Peer, Marty Perlmutar, NavindraPersaud, Andrew Pinto, Michelle Porepa, Nasreen Ramji, Noor Ramji, Alana Rosenthal, Janet Saunderson, Rahul Saxena, Michael Sgro, Susan Shepherd, Barbara Smiltnieks, Carolyn Taylor, Thea Weisdors, Sheila Wijayasinghe, Peter Wong, Ethel Ying, and Elizabeth Young.

ABBREVIATIONS

CBQ: Children’s Behavior Questionnaire; SD: Standard Deviation; OR: Odds Ratio

INTRODUCTION

Nutrition in early childhood has a significant impact on lifelong health and human development [1]. Nutrition experts recommend systematic screening before children are schoolaged, to identify risk factors and the need for preventive and early intervention services [2]. Nutrition risk is defined as the presence of characteristics or risk factors that can lead to negative nutritional outcomes [3]. The Nutrition Screening Tool for Every Pre- schooler (NutriSTEP ®) is a short questionnaire that can be completed by parent or caregivers of preschoolers in the community [4]. It has been validated to assess nutrition risk in preschool children and has questions in five domains: eating behavior, dietary intake, parental concerns about food and activity, screen time duration, and the use of supplements [4,5]. This questionnaire has been used to identify high nutrition risk (10-20%) in Canadian children [6,7].

Non-nutritional factors, such as individual child behavior characteristics known as temperament have been found to be associated with negative nutritional behavior and outcomes in young children [8,9]. Temperament refers to constitutionally based, individual differences in reactivity and self-regulation [10, 11]. Empirical evidence from developed countries have found infant’s with fussy-intense temperament to be associated with negative feeding behaviors such as shorter duration of breastfeeding, longer duration of bottle feeding and parentreported feeding problems [12-14]. Furthermore, difficult temperament has also been found to be associated with nutritional disorders, such as obesity and iron deficiency in children and neonates [15-20].

During the last few decades, researchers have developed scales to measure child temperament across a range of ages using parent reported and validated questionnaires [21,22]. The Children’s Behavior Questionnaire-Very Short Form (CBQ-VSF) is one such tool that provides a comprehensive assessment of reactive and self-regulative temperamental behavior patterns in pre-school children [11]. It identifies three highly differentiated temperament domains/constructs: Negative Affectivity, Effortful Control and Extraversion/Surgency. In light of the evidence indicating an association between child temperament and poor nutrition, we wanted to examine which of the temperament constructs identified by the CBQ-VSF are more likely to be associated with negative nutritional attributes identified as high nutritional risk in pre-school children.

Hence, the objective of our study was to examine whether an association exists between child temperament (measured using the CBQ) and higher nutrition risk (measured using the NutriSTEPTM) in pre-school children [4,11]. Establishment of such association will shed insight into the multifactorial and complex etiology of the development of nutritional disorders in young children. We also intended to gain a deeper understanding of this association by investigating the influence of potential effect moderators such as age, sex and socio-economic status [18].

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Participants

Healthy children aged 3-5 years were recruited from the community-based primary care research network for children in Toronto, Ontario (www.targetkids.ca) [23]. There are currently 10 group practices in the network, each of which has between 3 and 10 practicing physicians. Children with acute illness, birth weight less than 2500 gm, major congenital anomaly, clinically identifiable neuromotor delay and weight and height less than 2SD below the reference standard were excluded.

Study Design

In this cross-sectional study children attending their scheduled health supervision visits to participating TARGet Kids! Practice swere approached by research assistants to partake in the study. Parents were asked to complete a standardized survey instrument which collects health, nutritional and sociodemographic data. Parents also completed the NutriSTEPTM questionnaire and the CBQ-VSF [4,11,24]. Children and parents underwent anthropometric assessments using standardized instruments. All data generated from the TARGet Kids! Research initiative was entered into a web-based data management system (Medidata Rave®). From this electronic data capture and repository we were able to access all data collected through the TARGet Kids! Initiative.

Measures

The outcome for this study was nutrition risk, which was assessed using the NutriSTEPTM questionnaire [4]. This questionnaire consists of 17 items and each item has 2-5 response options. Responses are summed to provide an overall score (range of scores 0–68), where a higher score indicates increased nutrition risk. It has been recommended that a cut point of >25 for NutriSTEPTM should be referred to a dietitian for further assessment and treatment. Based on this cutoff, we categorized nutrition risk into two groups: high (NutriSTEPTMscore >25) and low (NutriSTEPTMscore ≤ 25) [4]. The NutriSTEPTM has been validated for use in a population of multicultural Canadian preschool-aged children, and found to have good correlation with nutrition risk assessed by a registered dietitian that included nutritional history and a 3-day dietary recall [4].

The main predictor, child temperament was assessed using the CBQ-VSF. This is a 36-item parent completed questionnaire developed for children aged 3-7 years [24]. The developers of the CBQ-VSF generated items to assess 15 selected temperament traits which were then factor analyzed to identify three broad temperament domains: Negative Affectivity, Effortful Control and Extraversion/Surgency. Table 1 describes the characteristics of the temperament domains. The CBQ-VSF has been validated in three different populations (US, Chinese and Japanese). All three populations showed similar reliability (mean 0.78) and internal consistency (0.68 to 0.93). Parental agreement on CBQ scales was substantial and consistent across samples. Comparison of parents’ report of the CBQ with laboratory observations of children’s behavior showed moderate correlation [11].

Important covariates which we considered as potential confounders included child age, sex and zBMI (calculated based on WHO growth standards), and maternal education. Evidence has shown child’s age, sex and maternal education may act as effect moderators to the temperament-nutrition linkage [18,25,26]. A moderator/effect modifier is an independent variable that affects the strength and/or direction of the association between another independent variable and an outcome variable [27]. Hence, we have also assessed the effect-modifying influence of these variables on the temperament and nutrition risk association.

Ethics approval for the study was obtained from Research Ethics Boards of The Hospital for Sick Children and St. Michael’s Hospital, Toronto, Canada.

Statistical Methods

Data was analyzed using the SAS version 9.1 (SAS Institute, Cary NC) and R version 3.0.1. Descriptive statistics (mean±SD; percentage) were used to describe the outcome and predictor variables. Characteristics were compared using the independent t test for continuous variables or the chi-square test for categorical variables. An unadjusted logistic regression analysis was performed to examine the association between each of the three temperament domains individually and the level of nutrition risk.

The primary analysis was an adjusted logistic regression analysis which examined the association between each of the three temperament domains and nutrition risk after controlling for variables that were a priori specified to have clinical importance and not highly correlated with each other (verified using variance inflation factor). This method avoids biased high correlation coefficient (R2 ) as well as biased low standard errors [28].

In a secondary analysis, effect-modification was assessed using interactions between each of the three broad temperament domains and child’s age, sex and maternal education in addition to all covariates included in the primary analysis model. Interactions were stated to be statistically significant both when tested simultaneously using a likelihood ratio test with a p-value of 0.3 as well as tested individually using a p-value of 0.05. In the multivariate analysis, multiple imputations were used to generate missing data (14%) using predictive mean matching with fifty datasets.

Table 1: Description of the temperament domains identified using the CBQ [23].

Temperament domain Description of the temperament domain
Higher score on Negative affectivity • Negatively affected by the sensory qualities of stimulus, such as pain, suffering, disappointment and object 
loss
• Show worry or nervousness in anticipation of pain or distress and/or to potentially threatening situation
• Becomes upset by interruption of on-going task or goal blocking
• Very difficult to soothe after events causing distress or excitement 
Higher score on Effortful control • Have pleasure or enjoy situation involving low stimulus intensity, rate, complexity or novelty. 
• Have the capacity to suppress inappropriate response to novel or uncertain situation, that is they have 
inhibitory control
• Maintain attentional focus on task-related channels
• Show positive affect (such as smiling or laughter) in response to changes in stimulus
Higher score on Extraversion/ Surgency • Impulsive
• Have pleasure or enjoy situation with high stimulus intensity, rate, complexity and novelty
• Have high level of activity involving gross motor movement
• Not shy, that is they do not show inhibited speed or discomfort to approach social situations
• Smile and laugh to appropriate social cues

 

RESULTS

A total of 3468 children aged 3-5 years, enrolled in the TARGet Kids! research initiative (July 2008 to August 2013) were assessed for their temperament and nutrition risk using the CBQ-VSF and NutriSTEPTM questionnaires. The sample had a mean (SD) age of 3.75 (0.56), zBMI of 0.29 (0.99), 52% were male and 92% of the mothers had college/university degree. Three independent scores for each of the three temperament domains were assessed for each child. The mean (SD) of the temperament domains Negative Affectivity, Effortful Control, and Extraversion/Surgency were 3.79 (0.85), 5.46 (0.72) and 4.56 (0.79) respectively. The level of nutrition risk identified using the NutriSTEPTM questionnaire showed 2996 (86%) children in the low risk and 472 (14%) in the high risk group. Table 2 shows the distribution of the predictors within the two nutrition risk groups. Significant difference was identified in respect to the age, sex, zBMI of the children and maternal education. In case of temperament, children in the high nutrition risk group scored significantly higher in the Negative Affectivity domain (p <0.0001), whereas had significantly lower scores in Effortful Control (p <0.0001).

Table 3 shows the results of the unadjusted and adjusted logistic regression analyses. Model 1 shows children with higher Negative Affectivity have an odds ratio (OR) of 1.47 (95% CI: 1.30, 1.66, p <0.0001) for high nutrition risk after adjusting for child age, sex, zBMI and mother’s education. Conversely, children with higher Effortful Control had an adjusted OR of 0.73 (95% CI: 0.64, 0.84, p <0.0001) for high nutrition risk (model 2). No significant association with Extraversion/Surgency and high nutrition risk was identified (model 3).

In a secondary analysis we included interaction between each of the three temperament domains and child’s age, sex and maternal education in addition to all covariates used in the primary analysis. None of these models showed significant interaction between temperament and child’s age, sex and maternal education.

DISCUSSION

The data presented here suggest a significant link between child temperament and nutrition risk. Negative Affectivity and Effortful Control, two temperament domains/constructs identified by the CBQ-VSF, were found to have significant association with nutrition risk in pre-school children. An explanation for this type of association may be provided at a biochemical level, where dietary dependent neurotransmitters involved in mood regulation, such as serotonin, have been shown to influence individual differences in the pattern of food consumption [10]. This evidence has led to the hypothesis that food preference patterns may reflect individuals’ attempts to self-regulate their own mood states. Individual level mechanisms that may explain these associations include the possibility that highly negative infants may have disorderly feeding behaviors and be less resistant to unnecessary food and may be overfed by their parents in response to their more frequent expressions of distress (“feeding to soothe”) [18].

We have also identified a differential effect of temperament on the development of nutrition risk in young children. Our results show children with a temperament that is consistent with Negative Affectivity are almost 1.5 times more at odds to have high nutrition risk. On the other hand, children displaying more of Effortful Control had 27% lower odds of having high nutrition risk. Temperament traits exhibited by Effortful Control such as positivity, able to control behavior, focus attention, follow parent requests and are pleasant to be with, may influence positive feeding behavior and environment and mitigate the development of high nutritional risk. In contract, temperament traits displayed by Negative Affectivity, such as discomfort, fear, anger/ frustration, sadness and poor soothability are conducive to the development of high level of nutrition risk in young children.

Our study proposes an association between individual differences in child temperament and high nutrition risk in young children. However, previous research has found that this association may also be moderated by specific child and family characteristics [26,29]. Hence, we examined the effect of specific child and family characteristics (age, sex and maternal education) on the temperament and nutrition risk association. However, our study did not show any significant effect modifying influence of these factors. In comparison to previous studies different measures of nutritional status and different age group of children used in the current study may explain this negative finding. In our future research with child temperament and nutrition, we plan to examine the effect of these variables using more objective measures of nutritional status (such as BMI) in a range of age groups of children.

Limitations of the current study relate to its cross-sectional nature which requires caution in regard to causal inferences. Since temporality of the cause and effect cannot be established using this study design, further research is needed to investigate the temporality of the association between Negative Affectivity and nutrition risk. Both NutriSTEPTM and CBQ-VSF are parent-reported which induces a certain level of ascertainment bias to the nutrition risk and temperament data. However, both NutriSTEPTM and CBQ-VSF have been shown to have good correlation with other objective methods that assess the same concept or construct in children [4,11].

Table 2: Distribution of clinically important variables within the two nutrition risk groups.

Predictors   Low nutrition risk N= 2996 High nutrition risk N= 472 p value
    Mean (SD) Frequency (%) Mean (SD) Frequency (%)
Age   3.74 (0.56) - 3.86 (0.56) - <.0001
Sex Female - 1484 (49.53 ) - 194 (41.10) 0.0007
Male - 1512 (50.47 ) - 278 (58.90 )
zbmi   0.32 (0.95) - 0.13 (1.25) - 0.0014
Negative Affectivity   3.74 (0.84) - 4.09 (0.88) - <.0001
Effortful Control   5.49 (0.71) - 5.28 (0.79) - <.0001
Extraversion/ Surgency   4.57 (0.80) - 4.53 (0.79) - 0.3224
Mothers education Elementary school - 15 (0.51) - 7 (1.51) <.0001
High school - 176 (6.04) - 86 (18.53)
College/ university - 2723(93.45) - 371 (79.96)

Table 3: Unadjusted and adjusted association between temperament domains and high nutrition risk in pre-school children.

  Predictor variable Unadjusted p-value Unadjusted OR (95% CI) Adjusted p-value Adjusted OR (95% CI)
Model 1 Negative Affectivity <.0001 1.63 (1.45, 1.8) <0.0001 1.47 (1.30, 1.66)
Model 2 Effortful Control <.0001 0.68 (0.60, 0.78) <.0001 0.73 (0.64, 0.84)
Model 3 Extraversion/ Surgency 0.3253 0.94 (0.83, 1.07) 0.2423 1.00 (0.81, 1.05)

 

CONCLUSION

Our study has identified the temperament trait of Negative Affectivity to have an increased likelihood of high nutrition risk in pre-school children. Even though more robust study design is needed to strengthen these associations, however, these concepts can alert physicians in clinical practice settings to behavior patterns in children that may predispose them to poor nutrition. This may guide clinical decision making, for example laboratory testing for nutritional status, counseling interventions and close monitoring.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

We acknowledge the support provided by the Canadian Institute of Health Research; Sick Kids Foundation; and St Michael’s Foundation. These funding organizations were not involved in design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis and interpretation of the data; and preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript.

REFERENCES

1. Ogata BN, Hayes D. Position of the Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics: nutrition guidance for healthy children ages 2 to 11 years. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2014; 114: 1257-1276.

2. Baer MT, Harris AB. Pediatric nutrition assessment: identifying children at risk. J Am Diet Assoc. 1997; 97: 107-115.

3. Identifying patients at risk: ADA’s definitions for nutrition screening and nutrition assessment. Council on Practice (COP) Quality Management Committee. J Am Diet Assoc. 1994; 94: 838-839.

4. Randall Simpson JA, Keller HH, Rysdale LA, Beyers JE. Nutrition Screening Tool for Every Preschooler (NutriSTEP): validation and test-retest reliability of a parent-administered questionnaire assessing nutrition risk of preschoolers. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2008; 62: 770-780.

5. Simpson JAR, Keller HH. NutriSTEP: Nutrition Screening Tool for Every Preschooler. Sudbury and District Health Unit. 2014.

6. Persaud N, Maguire JL, Lebovic G, Carsley S, Khovratovich M, Randall Simpson JA, et al. Association between serum cholesterol and eating behaviours during early childhood: a cross-sectional study. CMAJ. 2013; 185: 531-536.

7. Watson-Jarvis K, Fenton TR, McNeil D, Campbell K. Preschool nutrition risk in Calgary. Can J Diet Pract Res. 2011; 72: 101-106.

8. Wachs TD. Linking nutrition with temperament. In: Molfese VJ, Molfese DL, editors. Temperament and personality development across the life span. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 2000; 57-84.

9. Wachs TD. Multiple influences on children’s nutritional deficiencies: a systems perspective. Physiol Behav. 2008; 94: 48-60.

10. Rutter M. Temperament, personality and personality disorder. Br J Psychiatry. 1987; 150: 443-458.

11. Rothbart MK, Ahadi SA, Hershey KL, Fisher P. Investigations of temperament at three to seven years: the Children’s Behavior Questionnaire. Child Dev. 2001; 72: 1394-1408.

12. Thome M, Alder EM, Ramel A. A population-based study of exclusive breastfeeding in Icelandic women: is there a relationship with depressive symptoms and parenting stress? Int J Nurs Stud. 2006; 43: 11-20.

13. Hagekull B, Bohlin G, Rydell A. Maternal sensitivity, infant temperament and the development of early early feeding problems. Infant mental health journal. 1997; 1: 92-106.

14. Vandiver TA. Relationship of mothers’ perceptions and behaviors to the duration of breastfeeding. Psychol Rep. 1997; 80: 1375-1384.

15. Carey WB. Temperament and increased weight gain in infants. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 1985; 6: 128-131.

16. Carey WB, Hegvik RL, McDevitt SC. Temperamental factors associated with rapid weight gain and obesity in middle childhood. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 1988; 9: 194-198.

17. Darlington AS, Wright CM. The influence of temperament on weight gain in early infancy. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2006; 27: 329-335.

18. Anzman-Frasca S, Stifter CA, Birch LL. Temperament and childhood obesity risk: a review of the literature. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2012; 33: 732-745.

19. Wachs TD, Pollitt E, Cueto S, Jacoby E, Creed-Kanashiro H. Relation of neonatal iron status to individual variability in neonatal temperament. Dev Psychobiol. 2005; 46: 141-153.

20. Dietz WH Jr. Childhood obesity: susceptibility, cause, and management. J Pediatr. 1983; 103: 676-686.

21. Chess S, Thomas A. Temperament theory and practice. New York: Brunner/ Mazel. 1996.

22. Hubert NC, Wachs TD, Peters-Martin P, Gandour MJ. The study of early temperament: Measurement and conceptual issues. Chid Development. 1982; 53: 571-600.

23. Carsley S, Borkhoff CM, Maguire JL, Birken CS, Khovratovich M, McCrindle B, et al. Cohort Profile: The Applied Research Group for Kids (TARGet Kids!). Int J Epidemiol. 2015; 44: 776-788.

24. Putnam SP, Rothbart MK. Development of short and very short forms of the Children’s Behavior Questionnaire. J Pers Assess. 2006; 87: 102- 112.

25. Simonds JF, Simonds MP. Nursery school children’s temperament related to sex, birth position, and socioeconomic status. J Pediatr Psychol. 1982; 7: 49-59.

26. Mendlowicz MV, Jean-Louis G, Gillin JC, Akiskal HS, Furlanetto LM, Rapaport MH, et al. Sociodemographic predictors of temperament and character. J Psychiatr Res. 2000; 34: 221-226.

27. Bennett JA. Mediator and moderator variables in nursing research: conceptual and statistical differences. Res Nurs Health. 2000; 23: 415- 420.

28. Harrell FE. Regression Modeling Strategies. New York: Springer Science; 2001.

29. Hughes SO, Shewchuk RM. Child temperament, parent emotions, and perceptions of the child’s feeding experience. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2012; 9: 64.

Abdullah K, Lebovic G, Birken CS, Maguire JL, Jenkins J, et al. (2015) Association between Temperament and Nutrition Risk in Pre-School Children. Ann Pediatr Child Health 3(9): 1088.

Received : 21 Aug 2015
Accepted : 03 Nov 2015
Published : 05 Nov 2015
Journals
Annals of Otolaryngology and Rhinology
ISSN : 2379-948X
Launched : 2014
JSM Schizophrenia
Launched : 2016
Journal of Nausea
Launched : 2020
JSM Internal Medicine
Launched : 2016
JSM Hepatitis
Launched : 2016
JSM Oro Facial Surgeries
ISSN : 2578-3211
Launched : 2016
Journal of Human Nutrition and Food Science
ISSN : 2333-6706
Launched : 2013
JSM Regenerative Medicine and Bioengineering
ISSN : 2379-0490
Launched : 2013
JSM Spine
ISSN : 2578-3181
Launched : 2016
Archives of Palliative Care
ISSN : 2573-1165
Launched : 2016
JSM Nutritional Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3203
Launched : 2017
Annals of Neurodegenerative Disorders
ISSN : 2476-2032
Launched : 2016
Journal of Fever
ISSN : 2641-7782
Launched : 2017
JSM Bone Marrow Research
ISSN : 2578-3351
Launched : 2016
JSM Mathematics and Statistics
ISSN : 2578-3173
Launched : 2014
Journal of Autoimmunity and Research
ISSN : 2573-1173
Launched : 2014
JSM Arthritis
ISSN : 2475-9155
Launched : 2016
JSM Head and Neck Cancer-Cases and Reviews
ISSN : 2573-1610
Launched : 2016
JSM General Surgery Cases and Images
ISSN : 2573-1564
Launched : 2016
JSM Anatomy and Physiology
ISSN : 2573-1262
Launched : 2016
JSM Dental Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1548
Launched : 2016
Annals of Emergency Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1017
Launched : 2016
Annals of Mens Health and Wellness
ISSN : 2641-7707
Launched : 2017
Journal of Preventive Medicine and Health Care
ISSN : 2576-0084
Launched : 2018
Journal of Chronic Diseases and Management
ISSN : 2573-1300
Launched : 2016
Annals of Vaccines and Immunization
ISSN : 2378-9379
Launched : 2014
JSM Heart Surgery Cases and Images
ISSN : 2578-3157
Launched : 2016
Annals of Reproductive Medicine and Treatment
ISSN : 2573-1092
Launched : 2016
JSM Brain Science
ISSN : 2573-1289
Launched : 2016
JSM Biomarkers
ISSN : 2578-3815
Launched : 2014
JSM Biology
ISSN : 2475-9392
Launched : 2016
Archives of Stem Cell and Research
ISSN : 2578-3580
Launched : 2014
Annals of Clinical and Medical Microbiology
ISSN : 2578-3629
Launched : 2014
JSM Pediatric Surgery
ISSN : 2578-3149
Launched : 2017
Journal of Memory Disorder and Rehabilitation
ISSN : 2578-319X
Launched : 2016
JSM Tropical Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2578-3165
Launched : 2016
JSM Head and Face Medicine
ISSN : 2578-3793
Launched : 2016
JSM Cardiothoracic Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1297
Launched : 2016
JSM Bone and Joint Diseases
ISSN : 2578-3351
Launched : 2017
JSM Bioavailability and Bioequivalence
ISSN : 2641-7812
Launched : 2017
JSM Atherosclerosis
ISSN : 2573-1270
Launched : 2016
Journal of Genitourinary Disorders
ISSN : 2641-7790
Launched : 2017
Journal of Fractures and Sprains
ISSN : 2578-3831
Launched : 2016
Journal of Autism and Epilepsy
ISSN : 2641-7774
Launched : 2016
Annals of Marine Biology and Research
ISSN : 2573-105X
Launched : 2014
JSM Health Education & Primary Health Care
ISSN : 2578-3777
Launched : 2016
JSM Communication Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3807
Launched : 2016
Annals of Musculoskeletal Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3599
Launched : 2016
Annals of Virology and Research
ISSN : 2573-1122
Launched : 2014
JSM Renal Medicine
ISSN : 2573-1637
Launched : 2016
Journal of Muscle Health
ISSN : 2578-3823
Launched : 2016
JSM Genetics and Genomics
ISSN : 2334-1823
Launched : 2013
JSM Anxiety and Depression
ISSN : 2475-9139
Launched : 2016
Clinical Journal of Heart Diseases
ISSN : 2641-7766
Launched : 2016
Annals of Medicinal Chemistry and Research
ISSN : 2378-9336
Launched : 2014
JSM Pain and Management
ISSN : 2578-3378
Launched : 2016
JSM Women's Health
ISSN : 2578-3696
Launched : 2016
Clinical Research in HIV or AIDS
ISSN : 2374-0094
Launched : 2013
Journal of Endocrinology, Diabetes and Obesity
ISSN : 2333-6692
Launched : 2013
Journal of Substance Abuse and Alcoholism
ISSN : 2373-9363
Launched : 2013
JSM Neurosurgery and Spine
ISSN : 2373-9479
Launched : 2013
Journal of Liver and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2379-0830
Launched : 2014
Journal of Drug Design and Research
ISSN : 2379-089X
Launched : 2014
JSM Clinical Oncology and Research
ISSN : 2373-938X
Launched : 2013
JSM Bioinformatics, Genomics and Proteomics
ISSN : 2576-1102
Launched : 2014
JSM Chemistry
ISSN : 2334-1831
Launched : 2013
Journal of Trauma and Care
ISSN : 2573-1246
Launched : 2014
JSM Surgical Oncology and Research
ISSN : 2578-3688
Launched : 2016
Annals of Food Processing and Preservation
ISSN : 2573-1033
Launched : 2016
Journal of Radiology and Radiation Therapy
ISSN : 2333-7095
Launched : 2013
JSM Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation
ISSN : 2578-3572
Launched : 2016
Annals of Clinical Pathology
ISSN : 2373-9282
Launched : 2013
Annals of Cardiovascular Diseases
ISSN : 2641-7731
Launched : 2016
Journal of Behavior
ISSN : 2576-0076
Launched : 2016
Annals of Clinical and Experimental Metabolism
ISSN : 2572-2492
Launched : 2016
Clinical Research in Infectious Diseases
ISSN : 2379-0636
Launched : 2013
JSM Microbiology
ISSN : 2333-6455
Launched : 2013
Journal of Urology and Research
ISSN : 2379-951X
Launched : 2014
Journal of Family Medicine and Community Health
ISSN : 2379-0547
Launched : 2013
Annals of Pregnancy and Care
ISSN : 2578-336X
Launched : 2017
JSM Cell and Developmental Biology
ISSN : 2379-061X
Launched : 2013
Annals of Aquaculture and Research
ISSN : 2379-0881
Launched : 2014
Clinical Research in Pulmonology
ISSN : 2333-6625
Launched : 2013
Journal of Immunology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2333-6714
Launched : 2013
Annals of Forensic Research and Analysis
ISSN : 2378-9476
Launched : 2014
JSM Biochemistry and Molecular Biology
ISSN : 2333-7109
Launched : 2013
Annals of Breast Cancer Research
ISSN : 2641-7685
Launched : 2016
Annals of Gerontology and Geriatric Research
ISSN : 2378-9409
Launched : 2014
Journal of Sleep Medicine and Disorders
ISSN : 2379-0822
Launched : 2014
JSM Burns and Trauma
ISSN : 2475-9406
Launched : 2016
Chemical Engineering and Process Techniques
ISSN : 2333-6633
Launched : 2013
Annals of Clinical Cytology and Pathology
ISSN : 2475-9430
Launched : 2014
JSM Allergy and Asthma
ISSN : 2573-1254
Launched : 2016
Journal of Neurological Disorders and Stroke
ISSN : 2334-2307
Launched : 2013
Annals of Sports Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2379-0571
Launched : 2014
JSM Sexual Medicine
ISSN : 2578-3718
Launched : 2016
Annals of Vascular Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2378-9344
Launched : 2014
JSM Biotechnology and Biomedical Engineering
ISSN : 2333-7117
Launched : 2013
Journal of Hematology and Transfusion
ISSN : 2333-6684
Launched : 2013
JSM Environmental Science and Ecology
ISSN : 2333-7141
Launched : 2013
Journal of Cardiology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2333-6676
Launched : 2013
JSM Nanotechnology and Nanomedicine
ISSN : 2334-1815
Launched : 2013
Journal of Ear, Nose and Throat Disorders
ISSN : 2475-9473
Launched : 2016
JSM Ophthalmology
ISSN : 2333-6447
Launched : 2013
Journal of Pharmacology and Clinical Toxicology
ISSN : 2333-7079
Launched : 2013
Annals of Psychiatry and Mental Health
ISSN : 2374-0124
Launched : 2013
Medical Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology
ISSN : 2333-6439
Launched : 2013
JSM Clinical Pharmaceutics
ISSN : 2379-9498
Launched : 2014
JSM Foot and Ankle
ISSN : 2475-9112
Launched : 2016
JSM Alzheimer's Disease and Related Dementia
ISSN : 2378-9565
Launched : 2014
Journal of Addiction Medicine and Therapy
ISSN : 2333-665X
Launched : 2013
Journal of Veterinary Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2378-931X
Launched : 2013
Annals of Public Health and Research
ISSN : 2378-9328
Launched : 2014
Annals of Orthopedics and Rheumatology
ISSN : 2373-9290
Launched : 2013
Journal of Clinical Nephrology and Research
ISSN : 2379-0652
Launched : 2014
Annals of Community Medicine and Practice
ISSN : 2475-9465
Launched : 2014
Annals of Biometrics and Biostatistics
ISSN : 2374-0116
Launched : 2013
JSM Clinical Case Reports
ISSN : 2373-9819
Launched : 2013
Journal of Cancer Biology and Research
ISSN : 2373-9436
Launched : 2013
Journal of Surgery and Transplantation Science
ISSN : 2379-0911
Launched : 2013
Journal of Dermatology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2373-9371
Launched : 2013
JSM Gastroenterology and Hepatology
ISSN : 2373-9487
Launched : 2013
Annals of Nursing and Practice
ISSN : 2379-9501
Launched : 2014
JSM Dentistry
ISSN : 2333-7133
Launched : 2013
Author Information X