Loading

JSM Communication Disorders

Allophonic Theory of Dyslexia: A Short Overview

Review Article | Open Access

  • 1. Speech Perception Lab., CNRS & Paris Descartes University, France
+ Show More - Show Less
Corresponding Authors
Willy Serniclaes, Speech Perception Lab., CNRS & Paris Descartes University, 45, rue des Saints-Pères, 75006 Paris, France
Abstract

Dyslexia has three discernable sources: a visual deficit in the perception of letters, a phonological deficit in the perception of speech, and an audio-visual deficit that affects the association between letters with speech sounds. The phonological deficit in dyslexia might result from a specific mode of speech perception characterized by the use of allophonic units instead of phonemes. Here I summarize the available evidence in support of the “allophonic theory” of dyslexia. Different studies showed that the dyslexia deficit in the categorical perception of phonemic features (e.g., the place-of-articulation contrast between /b/ and /d/) is due to the enhanced sensitivity to allophonic features (e.g., the difference between two variants of /d/). A recent investigation showed that allophonic perception also gives rise to an enhanced sensitivity to allophonic segments, such as those that take place inside a consonant cluster. The implications of allophonic perception for the acquisition of the written language are discussed.

Keywords

Dyslexia; Reading; Speech perception; Allophonic theory

Citation

Serniclaes W (2018) Allophonic Theory of Dyslexia: A Short Overview. JSM Communication Dis 2(1): 1010.

ABBREVIATIONS

VOT: Voice Onset Time; F2: Second Formant; F3: Third Formant

INTRODUCTION

Developmental dyslexia is a deficit in the acquisition of written language, in the absence of other neuro-cognitive deficits. Dyslexia has a genetic basis with a fairly large prevalence (about 6% [1]). Both genetic and environmental factors determine the development of dyslexia and these factors become progressively clearer with the results of intensive research on reading and related processes in speech perception and letter vision.

Reading is basically a matter of associating letters with speech sounds. A child normally acquires written language by reading aloud, i.e. by producing the sounds that are represented by the letters, and by writing down the letters that correspond to speech sounds [2]. In the course of learning, reading becomes progressively silent, and the actual production or perceptions of speech sounds are replaced by their internal representations in phonological units. Phonological representations are activated by the visual representations of letters when reading and they activate graphical motor commands when writing.

THE THREE SOURCES OF DYSLEXIA

The acquisition of reading and writing rests on three basic components: the visual processing of letters (or other graphical symbols), the phonological representation of speech sounds and the association between letters and phonological units ([3] see Glossary). If each of these components contributes independently to the acquisition of written language, there should be three possible sources of dyslexia: a visual deficit in graphical processing, a phonological deficit in the processing of phonemes and other phonological units, and a phono-visual deficit in the set-up of the relationships between letters and phonemes. The examination of behavioral, neuro-physiological, and genetic evidence supports this view [3].

The three different core deficit scan be expressed in different ways, in terms of perceptual, attentional or short-term memory limitations [4], giving rise to a vast array of individual differences in the manifestations of dyslexia. Several different types of visual deficits seem to coexist, some being due to low-level processing [5], some others to different kinds of attentional problems [6,7], and still others in the visual representation of words[8]. Similarly, the phonological deficit surfaces in terms of perception [9], attention [10] and memory [11] limitations. These are different manifestations of the same core deficit which takes a different form depending on the individual developmental trajectory.

ALLOPHONIC THEORY

The very nature of the phonological deficit in dyslexia, beyond its different manifestations, remains debatable. However, there is growing evidence that such deficit arises from the representation of speech sounds in “allophonic” units ([12] see Glossary). According to the “allophonic theory”, the phonological deficit in dyslexia arises from a specific mode of speech perception that is characterized by the use of allophonic units, rather than phonological ones [13]. Allophonic units give a highly detailed description of speech sounds that is unnecessarily complex for accessing meaning. Even transparent writing systems - those with one-to-one relationships between phonemes and graphemes - are much too abstract for people who perceive speech with allophonic units, giving rise to major problems for the acquisition of the written language.

7. Glossary
Visual processing of letters: the ability to perceive, remember, and 
pay attention to relevant differences between letters.
Formant: a concentration of acoustic energy in some frequency band. Speech sounds are mainly characterized by three formants (F1, F2, F3), located in different frequency regions.
Phonological representation: mapping of speech sounds in phonological units (features and segments).
Phonological Features: elementary distinctions between speech sounds that pay independent contributions for distinguishing words and are specific to given language [15]. Examples (in English): the place-of-articulation feature differentiates sounds that are articulated at different places along the vocal tract (e.g. the b/d/g-like distinction between consonants with front/medial/back places of articulation); the voicing feature differentiates sounds that are articulated either with or without vibrations of the vocal folds (e.g. the b/p-like voiced-voiceless distinction between consonants).
Phonemes: phonological segments based on the conjunction of phonological features 15]. Examples: /b/ is both voiced and frontarticulated; /t/ is both voiceless and medial-articulated, etc.
Allophones: contextual variants of a phoneme that do not pay independent contributions for distinguishing words in a given language [25]. Examples: the differences in /g/ place-of-articulation between English words such as ‘good’, ‘great’ etc; differences in /p/ voicing between English words such as ‘pot’, ‘spot’ etc.
Allophonic Features: universal acoustic-auditory features that are combined for perceiving features in a given language [42]. Example: the difference in the direction of a single formant transition, either rising or falling.
ALLOPHONIC VS. PHONOLOGICAL UNITS

Reading and writing processes call upon phonological representations of speech sounds, whatever the writing system [14]. The most basic phonological units are distinctive features, i.e. distinctions between sounds that support differences in meaning [15]. A restricted set of some ten features such as “voicing” (e.g. d/t-like distinctions), “place-of-articulation” (e.g. b/d/g-like distinctions) … allow to generate ten thousands of different words by combinations in phonemes and concatenations of phonemes.

DEVELOPMENT OF PHONOLOGICAL REPRESENTATIONS

Phonological representations are the end-product of a longstanding developmental process. At the start, before some six months of age, the child is endowed with universal features that do not depend on language [16]. For instance, infants below one-year of age perceive the difference in the direction of a frequency transition, either rising or falling, much better than equivalent acoustic differences between transitions of the same category [17]. Such difference is “allophonic”: it contributes to the discrimination of articulatory movements, but it is not specific to language perception and is not specifically related to the phonology of a given language.

Allophonic features are integrated into language-specific distinctive features that fit into the phonological categories of the native language before the age of one year old [18]. Much later, not before the ages of five to six years old, phonological features are in turn grouped into phonemic segments, as evidenced by the increased precision in their perception [19-20] and the progressive apparition of phonemic awareness in pre-reading children [21].

ALLOPHONES VS. PHONEMES

The integration of universal features into language-specific ones and the grouping of the latter into phonemic segments face difficult challenges. Universal features cannot readily be used to generate the language-specific ones, and they must be “coupled” in specific ways to cope with the articulatory distinctions present in the language. Coupling means that the perception of one feature affects the perception of another one (“percept-percept” couplings: [22]). Similar difficulties arise for the concatenation of distinctive features into phoneme segments. Features correspond to different qualitative changes (i.e. placeof-articulation: change in the direction of frequency transitions) that are not synchronized in the acoustic signal, and phoneme perception can only be obtained by a temporal alignment of the features during neural processing.

Further difficulties in the build-up of distinctive features and their concatenation in phonemic segments arise from the occurrence of non-phonological features and segments in the speech signal. The coarticulation [23] between adjacent features gives rise to “allophones”, contextual variants of phonemes that do not contribute independently to separate the words in the language [24,25]. By extension,” allophonic features” correspond to distinctions between allophones. For instance, French voiceless stops can be produced as aspirated stops in some contexts, giving rise to a threefold voiced/voiceless/aspirated distinction, that is allophonic because voiceless/aspirated contrasts cannot be used alone to operate distinctions between French words [26]. However, such distinction is phonemic in some other languages (e.g. the threefold voiced/voiceless/aspirated distinction in Thaï).

The fact that features corresponding to a given phoneme are not synchronized in the acoustic speech signal can also create several allophonic segments in the same context, For instance in the French word /paRol/ (‘speech’) the frequency transitions that differentiate are not synchronized and the perceptual limits between /R/, /o/ and /l/ do not coincide with any of these transitions (Figure 1). Such discrepancies engender different vocalic segments, a prototypical /o/ surrounded by /Ro/ and /ol/ transitional segments. These two latter segments are acoustically different from the prototypical /o/ and correspond to vowels that might constitute separate phonemes in some other languages.

In summary, the development of phonological representations faces two obstacles. First, fairly complex combinations (perceptpercept couplings) between universal acoustic features must take place in order to generate language-specific phonological features. Second, the acoustic features are not synchronized in the speech signal and they need to be aligned during neural processing to create phonemic segments.

ALLOPHONIC PERCEPTION

Different studies suggest that people with dyslexia perceive speech sounds with allophonic units. Most studies have been devoted to the perception of allophonic features but a recent study deals with allophonic segments.

Dufor et al., using PET imaging were able to show that dyslexics subjects showed sensitivity to within phonemic category cues and brain activation enhancement in the left BA6 (premotor cortex in the frontal lobe) might suggest persistence of motor coding for allophonic representations of speech [33].

Sensitivity to Allophonic Features

Different stimulus continua were used to probe differences between people with dyslexia and typical readers in the perception of allophonic and phonemic boundaries. Voicing boundaries were investigated with VOT (“Voice Onset Time”; the time interval between the release of the oral closure and the onset of laryngeal pulsing [28]) continua. Discrimination and identification data collected with French children with dyslexia show that they are sensitive to a VOT boundaries that isallophonic in this language, whereas typical-reading controls are only sensitive to the phonemic VOT boundary [12,29]. However, children with dyslexia also perceive the phonemic VOT boundary but with a lesser precision than typical-reading children.

Place-of-articulation boundaries were investigated with F2-F3 stop-vowel transitions (frequency transitions between a stop consonant a vowel) continua, generated by systematic variation of the onset frequencies of the second and third formants. In a longitudinal study with Dutch children at familial risk for dyslexia, behavioral (discrimination and identification) data collected when the children were in the first grade showed that they were sensitive to an allophonic place-of-articulation boundary, whereas typical-reading controls were only sensitive to a phonemic boundary [30]. Such behavioral differences between groups were no more present when the children were in the second grade, but they were still present in neurophysiological responses [31]. Similar findings were evidenced for Dutch adults with dyslexia [32]. And a study with French adults with dyslexia evidenced an enhanced sensitivity to allophonic place-of-articulation contrasts in the left pre-motor cortex [33].

To sum up, the results collected in French and Dutch show that people with dyslexia: (1) are sensitive to allophonic features contrary to typical-reading controls; (2) still present such allophonic sensitivity in neural recordings after reading instruction, although it is sometimes absent in behavioral responses; (3) are also sensitive to phonemic boundaries but to a lesser extent than typical-readers.

Sensitivity to allophonic segments

Another prediction of allophonic perception is that it should also give rise to an enhanced sensitivity to allophonic segments, such as those that take place within a consonant cluster. In order to evidence an enhanced sensitivity to such allophonic variants, the duration of the acoustic segment between /R/ and /l/in the French word /paRol/ was reduced by segmenting out progressively larger portions starting from the middle [34]. As we have seen, segment between /R/ and /l/ contains a prototypical /o/ surrounded by /R/ and /l/ variants (Figure 1). If people with dyslexia indeed perceive the allophonic variants of /o/ as separate units, their paRol/ paRl boundary should be located at shorter durations along the continuum. This is indeed what was found. There was a fairly small (15 ms) but highly consistent difference in the location of the boundary, which afforded about 90% correct reclassification of the children in the dyslexic vs. control groups.

Finally, it remains possible that the enhanced sensitivity to the presence of a vocalic segment inside a consonant cluster might reflect a better temporal acuity, irrespective of allophonic status of the segment. The results of different studies indicate that people with dyslexia encode incoming information at higher cortical rates, providing a general framework to explain various phenomena, including the perception of allophonic features (“Temporal Sampling Framework” [35]) and sub-phonemic segments [36]. However, work in progress suggest that the enhanced sensitivity to short vocalic segments, as evidenced by differences in boundary location between children with dyslexia and controls on the paRol/paRl continuum, depends on their phonological status. No differences in boundary location between groups were found when the various segments that compose the natural segment in /paRol/ are replaced by a homogeneous /o/ segment (i.e. a segment with constant formant frequencies).

 

IMPLICATIONS OF ALLOPHONIC PERCEPTION FOR READING

In a study with English-speaking school-age children, neurophysiological responses to an allophonic durational contrast (atta/ata), that is phonemic in Finnish, were better correlated to reading skills than those with English place-of-articulation contrasts. Importantly, better performance with the foreign contrast was related to poorer reading skills [37].

Intervention studies that proceeded by training children to discriminate a d/t phonemic VOT contrast with a better precision evidenced effects on phonemic awareness skills (in children with dysphasia [38]) and on both phonemic awareness and reading skills (in children with dyslexia [39]).

However, the specific implications of allophonic perception for reading are still not entirely clear. The competition between allophonic and phonemic representations in children with dyslexia probably affects the synchronization of grapheme and phoneme decoding in the temporal cortex, which has critical importance for learning to read [40]. Allophonic decoding is faster than phonemic decoding in children with dyslexia, as evidenced by the latencies of the electrophysiological responses [31]. The fact that allophonic activation is faster than phonemic activation in children with dyslexia might prompt the synchronization between graphemes and allophones in the temporal cortex, at the detriment of the one between graphemes and phonemes.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

This work was supported by a public grant overseen by the French National Research Agency (ANR) as part of the “Investissementsd Avenir” program (reference: ANR-10- LABX-0083), and covered the costs to publish in open access. Many thanks to Cécile Houardher contribution to this paper.

REFERENCES

1. Peterson RL, Pennington BF. Developmental dyslexia. Lancet. 2012; 379: 1997-2007.

2. Share DL. Phonological recoding and self-teaching: sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition. 1995; 55: 219-226.

3. Serniclaes W, Sprenger-Charolles L. Reading impairment: From behavior to brain. In Handbook of Communication Disorders; Bahr R, Silliman E. Edn Routledge: London UK. 2015; 34-45.

4. Jones MR. Time, our lost dimension: toward a new theory of perception, attention, and memory. Psychol Rev. 1976; 83: 323-355.

5. Zorzi M, Barbiero C, Facoetti A, Lonciari I, Carrozzi M, Montico M, et al. Extra-large letter spacing improves reading in dyslexia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2012; 109: 11455-11459.

6. Facoetti A. Reading and selective spatial attention: evidence from behavioral studies in dyslexic children. In H.D. Tobias (Edn) Trends in dyslexia research. New York. Nova Science Publishers. 2004; 35-71.

7. Bosse ML, Tainturier MJ, Valdois S. Developmental dyslexia: the visual attention span deficit hypothesis. Cognition. 2007; 104: 198-230.

8. Cohen L, Dehaene S, Naccache L, Lehéricy S, Dehaene-Lambertz G, Hénaff MA, et al. The visual word form area: spatial and temporal characterization of an initial stage of reading in normal subjects and posterior split-brain patients. Brain. 2000; 123: 291-307.

9. Mody M, Studdert-Kennedy M, Brady S. Speech perception deficits in poor readers: auditory processing or phonological coding? J Exp Child Psychol. 1997; 64: 199-231.

10. Hazan V, Messaoud-Galusi S, Rosen S, Nouwens S, Shakespeare B. Speech perception abilities of adults with dyslexia: is there any evidence for a true deficit? J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2009; 52: 1510- 1529.

11. Ramus F, Szenkovits G. What phonological deficit? Q J Exp Psychol (Hove). 2008; 129-141.

12. Serniclaes W, Van Heghe S, Mousty P, Carr R, Sprenger-Charolles L. Allophonic mode of speech perception in dyslexia. J Exp Child Psychol. 2004; 87: 336-361.

13. Serniclaes W. Alophymic perception in dyslexia: a review. (Allophonic Perception in Dyslexia: An overview). In W. Serniclaes, Luque JL, Edn. Advances in research on evolutionary dyslexia. Writings Psychol. 2011; 25-34.

14. Perfetti CA, Zhang S, Berent I. Reading in English and Chinese: Evidence for a universal phonological principle. In Frost R, Katz L, Edn. Advances in psychology: Orthography phonology morphology and meaning Oxford England: North-Holland. 1992; 227-248.

15. Jakobson R, Fant G, Halle M. Preliminaries to speech analysis. The distinctive features and their correlates. Cambridge Mass. M.I.T. Press. 1952.

16. Werker JF. Tees RC. Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behav Dev. 1984; 7: 49-63.

17. Aslin RN. Discrimination of frequency transitions by human infants. J Acoust Soc Am. 1989; 86: 582-590.

18. Hoonhorst I, Colin C, Markessis E, Radeau M, Deltenre P, Serniclaes W. French native speakers in the making: from language-general to language-specific voicing boundaries. J Exp Child Psychol. 2009; 104: 353-366.

19. Hoonhorst I, Medina V, Colin C, Markessis E, Radeau M, Deltenre P, et al. The development of categorical perception: Comparisons between voicing colors and facial expressions. Speech Commun. 2011; 53: 417- 430.

20. Medina V, Hoonhorst I, Bogliotti C, Serniclaes W. Development of voicing perception in French: Comparing adults adolescents and children. J Phonetics. 2010; 38: 493-503.

21. Melby-Lervåg M, Lyster SA, Hulme C. Phonological skills and their role in learning to read: a meta-analytic review. Psychol Bull. 2012; 138: 322-352.

22. Epstein W. Percept-percept couplings. Perception. 1982; 11: 75-83.

23. Ohman SE. Coarticulation in VCV utterances: spectrographic measurements. J Acoust Soc Am. 1966; 39: 151-168. 24.Trask RL. A Dictionary of Phonetics and Phonology. UK. 1996. 25.Fudge EC. Phonology. Penguin: Harmonds worth UK. 1973.

26. Serniclaes W. Experimental study of the perception of the voicing trait of French occlusives. [Experimentalstudy of voicing perception in French stop consonants] PhD thesis in Psychological Sciences. Unpublished doctoral thesis. Free University of Brussels. 1987.

27. Boersma P, Weenink D. Praat: Doing Phonetics by Computer [Computer Program]. 2017. The Netherlands.

28. Lisker L, Abramson AS. A cross-language study of voicing in initial stops: acoustical measurements. Word. 1964; 20: 384-422.

29. Bogliotti C, Serniclaes W, Messaoud-Galusi S, Sprenger-Charolles L. Discrimination of speech sounds by dyslexic children: Comparisons with chronological age and reading level controls. J Exp Child Psychol. 2008; 101: 137-175.

30. Noordenbos MW, Segers E, Serniclaes W, Mitterer H, Verhoeven L. Allophonic mode of speech perception in Dutch children at risk for dyslexia: a longitudinal study. Res Dev Disabil. 2012; 33: 1469-1483.

31. Noordenbos MW, Segers E, Serniclaes W, Mitterer H, Verhoeven L. Neural evidence of allophonic perception in children at risk for dyslexia. Neuropsychologia. 2012; 50: 2010-2017.

32. Noordenbos MW, Segers E, Serniclaes W, Verhoeven L. Neural evidence of the allophonic mode of speech perception in adults with dyslexia. Clin Neurophysiol. 2013; 124: 1151-1162. 

33. Dufor O, Serniclaes W, Sprenger-Charolles L, Démonet JF. Left premotor cortex and allophonic speech perception in dyslexia: a PET study. Neuroimage. 2009; 46: 241-248

34. Serniclaes W, Seck M. Enhanced Sensitivity to Subphonemic Segments in Dyslexia: A New Instance of Allophonic Perception. Brain Sci. 2018.

35. Goswami U. A temporal sampling framework for developmental dyslexia. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011; 15: 3-10.

36. Giraud AL, Ramus F. Neurogenetics and auditory processing in developmental dyslexia. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2013; 23: 37-42.

37. Hamalainen J, Landi N, Loberg O, Lohvansuu K, Pugh K, Leppanen PHT. Brain event-related potentials to phoneme contrasts and their correlation to reading skills in school-age children. Int J Behav Dev. 2017.

38. Collet G, Colin C, Serniclaes W, Hoonhorst I, Markessis E, Deltenre P, et al. Effect of phonological training in French children with SLI: Perspectives on voicing identification discrimination and categorical perception. Res Dev Dis. 2012; 33: 1805-1818.

39. Collet G, Colin C, Serniclaes W. Audio-phonological Remediation of Dyslexia (RapDys?): Software to Change the Allophonic Perception of Speech Sounds in Phonemic Perception. ANAE [Audio-phonological remediation of dyslexia (RapDys?): software aimed to modify allophonic speech perception into phonemic perception]. 2017; 148: 01716486.

40. Blomert L. The neural signature of orthographic-phonological binding in successful and failing reading development. NeuroImage. 2011; 57: 695-703.

41. Serniclaes W. Features are phonological transforms of natural boundaries. In G.N. Clements and R. Ridouane, Edn. Where do phonological features come from? John Benjamins. 2011; 237-257

Received : 03 Apr 2018
Accepted : 25 May 2018
Published : 27 May 2018
Journals
Annals of Otolaryngology and Rhinology
ISSN : 2379-948X
Launched : 2014
JSM Schizophrenia
Launched : 2016
Journal of Nausea
Launched : 2020
JSM Internal Medicine
Launched : 2016
JSM Hepatitis
Launched : 2016
JSM Oro Facial Surgeries
ISSN : 2578-3211
Launched : 2016
Journal of Human Nutrition and Food Science
ISSN : 2333-6706
Launched : 2013
JSM Regenerative Medicine and Bioengineering
ISSN : 2379-0490
Launched : 2013
JSM Spine
ISSN : 2578-3181
Launched : 2016
Archives of Palliative Care
ISSN : 2573-1165
Launched : 2016
JSM Nutritional Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3203
Launched : 2017
Annals of Neurodegenerative Disorders
ISSN : 2476-2032
Launched : 2016
Journal of Fever
ISSN : 2641-7782
Launched : 2017
JSM Bone Marrow Research
ISSN : 2578-3351
Launched : 2016
JSM Mathematics and Statistics
ISSN : 2578-3173
Launched : 2014
Journal of Autoimmunity and Research
ISSN : 2573-1173
Launched : 2014
JSM Arthritis
ISSN : 2475-9155
Launched : 2016
JSM Head and Neck Cancer-Cases and Reviews
ISSN : 2573-1610
Launched : 2016
JSM General Surgery Cases and Images
ISSN : 2573-1564
Launched : 2016
JSM Anatomy and Physiology
ISSN : 2573-1262
Launched : 2016
JSM Dental Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1548
Launched : 2016
Annals of Emergency Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1017
Launched : 2016
Annals of Mens Health and Wellness
ISSN : 2641-7707
Launched : 2017
Journal of Preventive Medicine and Health Care
ISSN : 2576-0084
Launched : 2018
Journal of Chronic Diseases and Management
ISSN : 2573-1300
Launched : 2016
Annals of Vaccines and Immunization
ISSN : 2378-9379
Launched : 2014
JSM Heart Surgery Cases and Images
ISSN : 2578-3157
Launched : 2016
Annals of Reproductive Medicine and Treatment
ISSN : 2573-1092
Launched : 2016
JSM Brain Science
ISSN : 2573-1289
Launched : 2016
JSM Biomarkers
ISSN : 2578-3815
Launched : 2014
JSM Biology
ISSN : 2475-9392
Launched : 2016
Archives of Stem Cell and Research
ISSN : 2578-3580
Launched : 2014
Annals of Clinical and Medical Microbiology
ISSN : 2578-3629
Launched : 2014
JSM Pediatric Surgery
ISSN : 2578-3149
Launched : 2017
Journal of Memory Disorder and Rehabilitation
ISSN : 2578-319X
Launched : 2016
JSM Tropical Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2578-3165
Launched : 2016
JSM Head and Face Medicine
ISSN : 2578-3793
Launched : 2016
JSM Cardiothoracic Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1297
Launched : 2016
JSM Bone and Joint Diseases
ISSN : 2578-3351
Launched : 2017
JSM Bioavailability and Bioequivalence
ISSN : 2641-7812
Launched : 2017
JSM Atherosclerosis
ISSN : 2573-1270
Launched : 2016
Journal of Genitourinary Disorders
ISSN : 2641-7790
Launched : 2017
Journal of Fractures and Sprains
ISSN : 2578-3831
Launched : 2016
Journal of Autism and Epilepsy
ISSN : 2641-7774
Launched : 2016
Annals of Marine Biology and Research
ISSN : 2573-105X
Launched : 2014
JSM Health Education & Primary Health Care
ISSN : 2578-3777
Launched : 2016
Annals of Musculoskeletal Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3599
Launched : 2016
Annals of Virology and Research
ISSN : 2573-1122
Launched : 2014
JSM Renal Medicine
ISSN : 2573-1637
Launched : 2016
Journal of Muscle Health
ISSN : 2578-3823
Launched : 2016
JSM Genetics and Genomics
ISSN : 2334-1823
Launched : 2013
JSM Anxiety and Depression
ISSN : 2475-9139
Launched : 2016
Clinical Journal of Heart Diseases
ISSN : 2641-7766
Launched : 2016
Annals of Medicinal Chemistry and Research
ISSN : 2378-9336
Launched : 2014
JSM Pain and Management
ISSN : 2578-3378
Launched : 2016
JSM Women's Health
ISSN : 2578-3696
Launched : 2016
Clinical Research in HIV or AIDS
ISSN : 2374-0094
Launched : 2013
Journal of Endocrinology, Diabetes and Obesity
ISSN : 2333-6692
Launched : 2013
Journal of Substance Abuse and Alcoholism
ISSN : 2373-9363
Launched : 2013
JSM Neurosurgery and Spine
ISSN : 2373-9479
Launched : 2013
Journal of Liver and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2379-0830
Launched : 2014
Journal of Drug Design and Research
ISSN : 2379-089X
Launched : 2014
JSM Clinical Oncology and Research
ISSN : 2373-938X
Launched : 2013
JSM Bioinformatics, Genomics and Proteomics
ISSN : 2576-1102
Launched : 2014
JSM Chemistry
ISSN : 2334-1831
Launched : 2013
Journal of Trauma and Care
ISSN : 2573-1246
Launched : 2014
JSM Surgical Oncology and Research
ISSN : 2578-3688
Launched : 2016
Annals of Food Processing and Preservation
ISSN : 2573-1033
Launched : 2016
Journal of Radiology and Radiation Therapy
ISSN : 2333-7095
Launched : 2013
JSM Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation
ISSN : 2578-3572
Launched : 2016
Annals of Clinical Pathology
ISSN : 2373-9282
Launched : 2013
Annals of Cardiovascular Diseases
ISSN : 2641-7731
Launched : 2016
Journal of Behavior
ISSN : 2576-0076
Launched : 2016
Annals of Clinical and Experimental Metabolism
ISSN : 2572-2492
Launched : 2016
Clinical Research in Infectious Diseases
ISSN : 2379-0636
Launched : 2013
JSM Microbiology
ISSN : 2333-6455
Launched : 2013
Journal of Urology and Research
ISSN : 2379-951X
Launched : 2014
Journal of Family Medicine and Community Health
ISSN : 2379-0547
Launched : 2013
Annals of Pregnancy and Care
ISSN : 2578-336X
Launched : 2017
JSM Cell and Developmental Biology
ISSN : 2379-061X
Launched : 2013
Annals of Aquaculture and Research
ISSN : 2379-0881
Launched : 2014
Clinical Research in Pulmonology
ISSN : 2333-6625
Launched : 2013
Journal of Immunology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2333-6714
Launched : 2013
Annals of Forensic Research and Analysis
ISSN : 2378-9476
Launched : 2014
JSM Biochemistry and Molecular Biology
ISSN : 2333-7109
Launched : 2013
Annals of Breast Cancer Research
ISSN : 2641-7685
Launched : 2016
Annals of Gerontology and Geriatric Research
ISSN : 2378-9409
Launched : 2014
Journal of Sleep Medicine and Disorders
ISSN : 2379-0822
Launched : 2014
JSM Burns and Trauma
ISSN : 2475-9406
Launched : 2016
Chemical Engineering and Process Techniques
ISSN : 2333-6633
Launched : 2013
Annals of Clinical Cytology and Pathology
ISSN : 2475-9430
Launched : 2014
JSM Allergy and Asthma
ISSN : 2573-1254
Launched : 2016
Journal of Neurological Disorders and Stroke
ISSN : 2334-2307
Launched : 2013
Annals of Sports Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2379-0571
Launched : 2014
JSM Sexual Medicine
ISSN : 2578-3718
Launched : 2016
Annals of Vascular Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2378-9344
Launched : 2014
JSM Biotechnology and Biomedical Engineering
ISSN : 2333-7117
Launched : 2013
Journal of Hematology and Transfusion
ISSN : 2333-6684
Launched : 2013
JSM Environmental Science and Ecology
ISSN : 2333-7141
Launched : 2013
Journal of Cardiology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2333-6676
Launched : 2013
JSM Nanotechnology and Nanomedicine
ISSN : 2334-1815
Launched : 2013
Journal of Ear, Nose and Throat Disorders
ISSN : 2475-9473
Launched : 2016
JSM Ophthalmology
ISSN : 2333-6447
Launched : 2013
Journal of Pharmacology and Clinical Toxicology
ISSN : 2333-7079
Launched : 2013
Annals of Psychiatry and Mental Health
ISSN : 2374-0124
Launched : 2013
Medical Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology
ISSN : 2333-6439
Launched : 2013
Annals of Pediatrics and Child Health
ISSN : 2373-9312
Launched : 2013
JSM Clinical Pharmaceutics
ISSN : 2379-9498
Launched : 2014
JSM Foot and Ankle
ISSN : 2475-9112
Launched : 2016
JSM Alzheimer's Disease and Related Dementia
ISSN : 2378-9565
Launched : 2014
Journal of Addiction Medicine and Therapy
ISSN : 2333-665X
Launched : 2013
Journal of Veterinary Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2378-931X
Launched : 2013
Annals of Public Health and Research
ISSN : 2378-9328
Launched : 2014
Annals of Orthopedics and Rheumatology
ISSN : 2373-9290
Launched : 2013
Journal of Clinical Nephrology and Research
ISSN : 2379-0652
Launched : 2014
Annals of Community Medicine and Practice
ISSN : 2475-9465
Launched : 2014
Annals of Biometrics and Biostatistics
ISSN : 2374-0116
Launched : 2013
JSM Clinical Case Reports
ISSN : 2373-9819
Launched : 2013
Journal of Cancer Biology and Research
ISSN : 2373-9436
Launched : 2013
Journal of Surgery and Transplantation Science
ISSN : 2379-0911
Launched : 2013
Journal of Dermatology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2373-9371
Launched : 2013
JSM Gastroenterology and Hepatology
ISSN : 2373-9487
Launched : 2013
Annals of Nursing and Practice
ISSN : 2379-9501
Launched : 2014
JSM Dentistry
ISSN : 2333-7133
Launched : 2013
Author Information X