Loading

Journal of Behavior

A Redefinition of Facial Communication in Non-Human Animals

Review Article | Open Access | Volume 3 | Issue 1

  • 1. Faculty of Medecine, Université libre de Bruxelles, Belgium
+ Show More - Show Less
Corresponding Authors
Claude Tomberg, Faculty of Medecine, Université libre de Bruxelles, Belgium, 808, route de Lennick, CP 630, Brussels, Belgium, Tel: +32-02-555.64.05
Abstract

In humans, social communication is mostly conveyed by facial expressions, which are widely shared among Mammals. Based on current knowledge, we explore the concept of facial communication from an evolutionary point of view and examine how far it might not only be performed by Mammals, but more broadly by Amniotes. As we investigate facial communication in various species, we find out that facial expressions are restrained to Mammals. However, even if non-mammals lack of cutaneous facial muscles responsible of facial expressions, they display facial signals bearing a communicative value. Thus, facial communication is not clustered to Mammals. Moreover, some facial displays are shared by almost every Amniotes, as the eye-blink which has been suggested to be related to social factors aside its physiological role. Yet, to understand the terminology of this research field, definitions should be unified. Thus, based on current data on Amniotes’ facial communication, we proposed extended definitions of facial movements, behaviours and expressions: movements are visible displacements of body segments or tissues. They are motor action that do not need cognitive and emotional implication, while behaviours require the interpretation of environmental or internal stimuli, as the presence of an audience or emotional experiences. Behaviours are movements that can be involved in social communication. So facial behaviours are not cluster to Mammals but might be also expressed by other Amniote’s species. Whereas facial expressions are facial behaviours generated by the contraction of cutaneous facial muscles, innervated by the cranial nerve VII, and thus only expressed by Mammals.

Keywords

•    Facial expressions
•    Facial behaviors
•    Facial movements
•    Social communication
•    Amniotes
•    Cutaneous muscles
•    Cranial nerve

Citation

Pellon S, Hallegot M., Lapique J. and Tomberg C (2020) A Redefinition of Facial Communication in Non-Human Animals. J Behav 3(1): 1017

INTRODUCTION

The face supports multiple functions as hearing, vision, breathing, feeding and social communication. In Mammals for example, facial expressions may display emotions and intentions of an individual [1,2] with a variable accuracy depending on the species. Several definitions of facial expressions have been proposed, sharing similarities but lacking precisions. According to Ekman [3], facial expressions are universal signals reflecting individuals’ emotional states. Ying-Li Tian added that these signals can be used in social communication [4], and Harley associated facial expressions to underlying muscles [5]. Waller and colleagues [6] suggested that facial “expressions” should not be used and instead proposed facial “behaviours”. They defined it as “observable facial movements associated with the typical behavioral repertoire of a species that potentially have communicative meaning to conspecifics.” As for facial expressions, the term “behaviour” has a massive amount of definition, which varies between almost every author [7-9]. Yet, Levitis and colleagues [10] proposed a definition based on the reviewing and polling of behavioural scientists: they defined it as “the internally coordinated responses (actions or inactions) of whole living organisms (individuals or groups) to internal and/or external stimuli, excluding responses more easily understood as developmental changes.” As the term “expression” or “behaviour” may drive emotional or communicative content, the neutral term “movement” will be used in this article to evaluate the various aspects of facial communication.

Studies of facial expressions started at the end of the 19th century [11,12] but a new impetus has been given during the 70’s with Ekman’s proposal of the Facial Action Coding System (FACS), allowing a unified analysis of human facial expressions based on their facial musculature [13]. Studies on animals were not abundant until the beginning of the 21st century, when research on animals’ facial communication gained interest. FACS started to be adapted for animals, first in Primates as chimpanzees [14], hylobatids [15], orangutans [16] and macaques [17], then in domestic animals with dogs [18], horses [19] and cats [20]. At the same time, studies focused on animal welfare created pain scales in many species: in mice [21], rats [22], rabbits [23], horses [24], sheeps [25,26], pigs [27], goats [28], ferrets [29], seals [30], cattles [31] and cats [32]. This increase of studies about facial communication in the last decade requires the unification of the terms used in this field.

This review focus on facial movements involved in the production of communicative signals in Amniotes in general, and not only mammals in a wider evolutionary perspective, as excluding other species than mammals may lead to underestimate the importance of facial movements in social communication and to miss information about their evolution. How far facial movements could be used as indicators of communicative abilities in non-human species is not so clear, except for the anthropoids. As many Amniotes species express behaviours related to emotions [33], they might express communicative and emotional content by facial movements too, as it is assumed to be for facial expressions in humans [5]. This evolutionary approach requires to clarify the definition of facial expression and behaviour based on the Amniotes’ clade. It will be discussed by looking at anatomical, neurobiological, evolutionary and socio-ethological features of facial movements across Amniotes’ species.

Facial movements in Mammals

Neuroanatomical features of facial movements: In addition to masticatory muscles and with few exceptions, Mammals’ head possess cutaneous muscles –also called facial muscles- originating from bones or fibrous structures, they are the only group of muscles inserting into the skin [34]. Their contractions create lines, folds and wrinkles [35]. They have specific characteristics compared to skeletal muscles of the limbs and the trunk [36–38]. They are striated with a larger percentage of slow type fibers but possess Ruffini-like corpuscles for proprioceptive functions [36,39] instead of muscle spindles and Golgi’s tendon organs [36–38,40].

However, non-cutaneous muscles as masticator muscles also contribute to some facial movements described in FACS, as Jaw drop (Action Unit (AU) 26 [14,18,20]), through the action of the digastric and masseter muscles.

Moreover, specific muscles have been newly developed along with domestication as the levator anguli occuli medialis in dogs, not found in wolves [41–43]. This muscle raises the inner eyebrow (AU101). Interestingly, dogs expressing more AU101 are adopted faster [18], suggesting a selective advantage because of an infant-like display.

The embryogenic origin of the head and the neck tissues are common portions of the embryonic foregut. There are five pairs of pharyngeal arches in all Amniotes [44,45]. From the first arch will develop the jaws and the masticatory muscles along with the trigeminal nerve (cranial nerve V (CNV)) whose V3 branch innervates them. Facial cutaneous muscles of Mammals are a subgroup of hyoid muscles, and they arise from the second pharyngeal arch in gestation together with the facial nerve (CNVII) [46,47].

Facial cutaneous muscles are innervated by the CNVII [34], except the levator palprebrae superioris which is innervated by the CN III [48] (the oculomotor nerve). This nerve is derived from the basal plate of the embryonic midbrain. The retractor bulbi, a non-cutaneous muscle implied in eye movements, is innervated by the CN VI [49]. Muscles implied in head movements are non-cutaneous and present a similar pattern in most mammals [50]. They are innervated mostly by CN XI and cervical spinal nerve [50].

In humans, 24 facial muscles contribute to facial expressions [51,52]. Remarkably, there are few variations of this amount between species (Table 1). Rodents, such as rats (Rattus norvegicus), have up to 24 facial muscles, whereas 23 are described in Macaca mulatta, 21 in Lepilemur ruficaudatus, 22 in Tupaia, 22 in Pan troglodytes, 24 in Gorilla gorilla, 21 in Pongo pygmaeus, 23 in Hylobates [53]. Yet, some species possess less muscles, as dogs and horses which, besides ears muscles, have both 16 muscles [34] and 18 in cats [20]. Similar number of muscles is coherent with distribution of neuroanatomical traits that appear to have been highly conserved through phylogenetic history [54] with few variabilities within clades [55]. Meanwhile, some muscles may be conserved, absent or vestigial in some taxa as a result of their specific evolutionary process [56]. However, the number of facial muscles is not the only factor of facial movement’s complexity. Their combined actions with small anatomical, physiological or biochemistry variations may produce a larger variety of facial movements between taxa [57– 59].

Brainstem nuclei may have evolved with ecological adaptations and reflect specializations of peripheral structures [60–65]. A strong relationship has been found between the size of neural and muscular structures and their use frequency [66]. For example, among the Apes, the larger species have a larger facial nucleus and display more expressive faces than the smaller one [67,68]. Primates with larger facial nuclei tend to have higher differentiation of facial muscles which may reflect a greater use of vision in social communication [69]. Several anthropoids have direct cortico-motoneuronal innervation to facial nerve. This allows higher accuracy in voluntary motor command of facial muscles [70–78], while in non-anthropoid Mammals axonal projections are polysynaptic via synaptic connections in the parvocellular reticular formation [79,80].

It should be noted that the facial nerve’s enlargement may be observed in species which do not rely substantially on visual communication for social interactions, like the nocturnal prosimians Tarsius and Otolemur [69,81]. In Tarsius syrichta, the great volume of the facial nerve is probably related to the orbicularis oculi and depressor palpebrae inferioris whose fibers’ structure is specialized to protect the exceptionally large eyeball [69,82].

Cetaceans are different from the other Mammals, even if they are phylogenetically Artiodactyla [83], they live in an aquatic environment [84]. One of their environmental adaptation is a “low” facial mobility due to modifications or disappearances of facial muscles [85]. The muscles mobilizing the eyelids are the only ones keeping a similar role with the terrestrial Mammals [85]. Cetaceans have also a massive retractor bulbi muscle [86], present in Reptilians, Birds and almost all Mammals [86,87]. In these last species, its function is to move the nictitating membrane, the so-called “third eyelid” [88] but Cetaceans do not have one [89]. However, their well-developed retractor bulbi, used to retract the eye into the orbit [84], may help to close hardly mobile and particularly thick eyelids filled with a high amount of fat. Some Cetaceans have developed a specific moveable structure on their nose, the melon [91,92], which seems to be a communicative tool (orientation, reception and emission of sound) in beluga [90]. Moreover, some species perform some facial movements, as belugas can alter the shape of their mouth, without a clear communicative value identified, whereas dolphins express the “jaw clapping” which seems to be an antagonist signal [90,93].

Ecological influence on facial movements: The body size seems correlated to facial mobility in anthropoid monkeys [68], as a small body size may constrain the evolution of facial movements. However, this strict linear relation might be too restrictive as gorillas and chimpanzees present a similar number of AUs in their respective FACS but have different body size [94].

Moreover, body size is correlated to visual acuity, as eye axial length depends of the body size [95–97]. Eye size determines the distance between the lens and retina, which implies the retinal image size [98] and consequently its precision [99]. A good visual acuity allows the use of accurate visual cues to communicate [100], whereas species with poor visual acuity might develop other communicative pathways, such as echolocation for Chiropterans [101]. Thus, visual acuity might influence the diversity of facial expressions. Yet, this hypothesis is not supported by findings on other species than Primates, as species with poor visual acuity such as rats (0.95 c/deg [102]) or cows (1.6 c/deg [103]) perform a diversity of facial movements. On the other hand, some Cetaceans have a good visual acuity (~25 c/deg in dolphin [104]) but lack facial mobility. Yet, to date, no study directly addresses this question.

Obstruction of the visual field (by trees for example) does not seem to influence the production of facial movements either, as arboreal Primates do not produce fewer facial movements than terrestrial ones [94,105]. A similar idea would be that nocturnal animals display fewer facial movements than diurnal ones, and indeed, nocturnal animals perform less eye blinks [105,106]. Nevertheless, cats are mainly nocturnal animals, and they have the greatest amount of AU described in animal FACS, including ears and whiskers movements [107],[108]. However, it is not clear how far these movements disclose communicative, cognitive, or ecological values, such as visual perception, sound localisation and tactile perception of surrounding.

Facial movements help to handle conflict or to regulate social bond [109–113]. For example, Apes live in big groups [113,114] and express complex facial displays in social interactions [15,16,113,115]. Various facial movements are displayed regarding either aggressive, submissive or affiliative situations, as the “silent bared teeth” in Primates [14], [116,117]. Moreover, Dobson [94] demonstrated that “social group is a good predictor of facial mobility”. The difference between gorillas and chimpanzees could be clarified regarding their sociality, as chimpanzees live in big groups whereas gorillas live in small or dyadic groups [118]. Mating systems might also influence social interactions complexity [119] and brain size, more specifically the size of the cerebral areas involved in face processing [1,2,120]. Gibbons and siamangs are monogamous species and have fewer facial cutaneous muscles than their close relatives, chimpanzees [121,122]. The GibbonFACS describes fewer action units [15]. It has been suggested that monogamy induces less opportunities for social interactions and that the extension of facial movements in bigger groups could result of a necessity to increase combination of displays or to enhance understanding of signals, thus group cohesion [94].

In opposition, some solitary animals present various facial movements. Orangutans perform a wide repertoire of facial movements, almost as important as social Primates [16]. Sun bears display various types of open-mouth expressions during social play, even if they rarely take part into interactions with congeners [123]. The authors suggested that their facial expressions might not have evolved in adaptation to “complex social environment”.

Behavioral evolution: Facial movements allow individuals to share their internal states with congeners [124–127]. In this matter, facial movements are a useful tool in social interactions and even slight changes can display different intensities for the same state [116,128]. Thus, facial movements are accurate visual cues to measure emotions. The negative valence has been more studied, and many mouth movements can express negative emotions [58,58,125,129–133]. But tiny variations can change the meaning, as open mouth with exposed teeth is a sign of fear in Primates [58,129,134], but during positive contexts, the same movement without teeth exposure occurred. Some movements are also associated with positive emotions: dogs’ ears get closer during positive situations [131], horses’ ears are pulled back during a pleasant event [127] or chimpanzees’ head movements are displayed during affiliative interactions [135]. Yet, the most investigated state is pain thanks to several pain scales which allow scientists to monitor animals’ welfare. The main share changes observed in many species, that might indicate a common origin, are nose wrinkling, orbital tightening, ears flattening, open mouth, reduction of distance between ears and whiskers retraction [21–27,29,32,128,136–138].

It is important to keep in mind that AUs do not bear a signification per se but composed faces are meaningful, even if some movements are more expressed during specific situations such as stress or relaxation.

Facial movements might have evolved from physiological role to communicative tools, e.g. ears flattening might have firstly belong to a startle reflex developing later a communicative value [139]. The nose wrinkle and upper lid raiser, performed during fear, were displayed originally to protect sensitive regions (mouth, nose, eyes) from biting. Darwin suggested that disgust face helped to protect from toxic substance [140], and later evolved as a communicative tool. Moreover, some scientists proposed that the actual anger face might be a vestigial version of aggressive biting behaviours, without proof supporting this theory [141]. Nowadays, in Primates, open-mouth without exposed teeth is observed in pleasant contexts [58,129,134], and bears also performed it during play situation [123]. Primates relaxed open-mouth -supposed to be the premise of the human smile- is a non-aggressive play signal, as the silent bared-teeth which is assimilated to smiling and laughing in humans [142,143]. This supposes that these displays are dissociated from their original function. This hypothesis is supported by the research of Davila-Ross [144], as chimpanzees open-mouth face is produced during social play and can be associated with laugh vocalizations. Yet, Canids’ horizontal and vertical lip retraction have opposite signification, respectively a submissive greeting and dominant threat [145]. Thus, facial movements’ signification varied among species, even if they share similar evolutionary patterns in Primates.

Primates can perform facial movements as soon as they are born, with smile, anger and innate movements for bitter tastes [1,146][147,148]. Facial movements seem to be shaped by mimicry of parent facial displays [149]. Exposition to facial movements of conspecifics help to develop both recognition and production of facial movements [1].

The importance of facial movements in social context can be illustrated by the “audience effect”, defined as changes in individuals’ behaviour observed by another [150], for example an increase of facial expressions in humans [151–153]. A similar phenomenon has been described in various species, such as gibbons [154], orangutans [155] and dogs when humans face them [156]. In horses, results are less clear, as an observer presence effect has been described for global behaviour [157– 159] but not for facial movements -in a pain context [138]. But in another context, horses expressed differently facial expressions while an experimenter is present [160].

Facial movements in non-mammal animals

Anatomical features of facial movements: Non-mammal animals lack cutaneous facial muscles, and their rigid facial mask prevents the production of facial movements [161]. Yet, Birds possess orbicularis oculi muscles similar to the Mammals’ one, as the depressor palpabrae ventralis whose relaxation allows Birds to close their eyes [162]. Reptiles move their eyelids thanks to the depressor palpebrae inferioris [163]. Indeed, Birds’ and Reptiles’ upper eyelid is thick, less mobile than the lower one. Moreover, Reptiles possess the retractor bulbi, absent in Birds, which acts as in Mammals [164], [162].

Reptiles possess jaw muscles similar to Mammals’ ones, as the levator anguli oris or pterygoid muscles [165–168] [169]. Within the 8 muscles implied in beak movements in Birds [169], three are shared with Reptiles [165,169,170]. These muscles are used in various situation: agonistic behaviours or sexual parades [171,172]. Reptiles neck muscles are similar to Mammals’ one [173–175], whereas Birds’ neck muscles are mainly different [176]. They allow to move the head and take part into various choreography [172,177,178]. Lacking external ears, Sauropsida lack ears muscles, as they lack nose muscles. To date, no study references the number of facial muscles in neither Birds nor Reptiles.

Even if many reptiles as turtles, snakes and crocodiles use mainly olfactive and tactile communication, some of them, especially lizards, disclose a large repertoire of visual communicative tools [179], as body movements [180]. Where Birds present feathers crown or colour spots [181], some lizards possess a dewlap for Anolis (extensible flap of skin under the throat) or a frill (an erectile throat fan around the neck) as facial ornaments [177,180], [177]. The dewlap and the frill are supported by the hyoid apparatus [177,182] and dewlap movements are generated by the ceratohyoid muscle [182].

The hyoid apparatus, including the ceratohyoid muscle mobilizing the dewlap, is a derivative of the 2nd pharyngeal arch, [182], like Mammals’ facial muscles. Non-mammal’s similar to or shared muscles with Mammal’s are innervated by CN V like the orbicularis oculi in Birds [162,183] or the levator anguli oris in lizards [184], instead of the CN VII in Mammals [34], except for the depressor mandibulae in Reptiles [185]. Some muscles are innervated by the CN VI as the retractor bulbi in Reptiles and Mammals, absent in Birds [49,162,164]. In non-mammals, the CN VII role is to innervate glands [162], the choroid [186] and some of its fibers “mixed” with the CN V [162].

As in Mammals where the size of facial nucleus varies with facial movements repertoire, in songbirds high vocal center and robustus archistriatalis nucleus size are correlated with song repertoire [187] [188]. Yet, the direction of the correlation is unknown, and either it could be the number of songs which determines the size of the nuclei, or the size of the nuclei which determines the number of songs learned. More generally, brain size in Birds has been linked to sociality [189] and body weight [190]. In lizards, frequency of use and size of dewlap are correlated. The size of the muscle fibers and the motoneurons innervating the cerato-hyoideus muscle are correlated to the size of the dewlap [191].

Ecological influence on facial movements: Birds’ brains, as Mammals’ ones, are bigger than Reptiles’ and Amphibians’ brains. This difference is particularly noticeable in songbirds and Primates, and they are groups which communicate the most through long distances thanks to visual and vocal pathways [190].

Birds’ eyes are proportionally bigger than Mammals’ ones compared to their body size [192]. Moreover, Birds -and especially raptors- have among the best visual acuity in animal reign, reaching 140c/deg in eagles [193], or 73 c/deg in falcon [194]. Indeed, Birds rely mainly on visual information in order to scan their environment [192], contrary to Reptiles [179].

Various ecological factors are related to visual acuity as hunt distance in Birds [195] and predators’ detection [196]. Blackwell and colleagues [196] did not manage to establish a correlation between visual acuity and behavioural responses, but they suggested that visual characteristics might influence behavioural responses and risk evaluation.

Reptiles have a low visual acuity, around 1.2 c/deg for Anolis [197]. Regarding to other Reptiles, some species can reach up to 6.8 c/deg or 4.9 c/deg [198]. Another study points that Atlantic green turtle is myopic out of water [199], contrary to Cetaceans which have a better view in air than in water. To date, no study mentioned a possible correlation between visual acuity, body size and facial movements in Birds or Reptiles.

As Mammals, Birds eye-blink rates varied upon nocturnal and diurnal species. Indeed, diurnal species blink more than nocturnal ones [106], even if blinking might not be related to environmental properties as temperature and wind speed [200].

Both diurnal and nocturnal bird species modify their foraging behaviours at night, as diurnal Birds can be active at night too [201]. Because of the lower visibility, they perform shorter movements [202]. To our knowledge, no study mentioned behavioural differences between nocturnal and diurnal birds or reptiles. It might be because Birds and Reptiles, as Anolis, are mainly diurnal [203], [204], even if some species can be nocturnal [205–207].

In Birds and Reptiles, some facial or head movements have communicative values that might influence group cohesion. For example, the beak is involved in both agonistic behaviours (peaking opponent) and affiliate ones (allopreening for example) and so, is a key feature of Bird’s communication [171,208,209]. The ruffling of Birds’ feathers is associated with aggression, on the other hand the slicking of feather is an avoidance behaviour [161,210]. In lizards, the headbob of the Anolis or the dewlap movements are used to communicate with congeners and maintain or reject interactions [177,178].

Each movement can convey a specific or several significations, as food-solicitating or stressful events with head bobbing in parrots [211]. On the opposite, lizards’ dewlap and frill provide information about individuals’ gender and is used in competition for resources, sexual partners and preservation of territories, especially between males [178–180,212]. These movements among others are performed by head features, and thus might be considered as facial movements.

Mating style seems to be linked to communication complexity [119], but as Birds are mainly monogamous and reptiles polygamous [213], it would be difficult to investigate a potential effect on their facial movements. However, in Birds, extrapair copulation are observed and can even lead to a “divorce” [214,215]. Moreover, a positive correlation has been found between brain size and pair bond strength, and the authors suggested it might be the result of higher cooperation and negotiation [189]. These interactions could be based on specific head movements, but no study investigated yet their potential role on the stability of pair cohesion.

However, sociality alone cannot explain signal complexity. A study found a correlation between social factors and signal complexity only in lizards, with sexual size dimorphism correlated to ornaments, color and headbobs [216]. In Birds, ecological factors and allometry seem to be good explanatory factors to signal complexity. Ecology can influence sociality: because of luminosity, obstructions and interferences by acoustic or visual backgrounds which can mask signals, animals adopt strategies to enhance communication signals. Some lizards for example, which switched from terrestrial to arboreal lifestyle, developed more elaborate territorial displays [216].

Behavioral evolution: In Birds, some behaviour might have evolved from thermoregulation to communicative signal. Feathers erections have an agonistic value, but they also can indicate health status to conspecifics [210], as Birds appears bigger, they also seem more threatening. In lizards, no study investigated in our knowledge significance evolution of their head movements.

Birds behaviours are influenced by the social context. The most relevant example might be crows and jays caching behaviours. These species pay attention to both auditory and visual presence of competitors. Jays adapt their caching location in order to cache their food at a greater distance of their conspecifics [217]. However, the recovery of food items is not influenced by the presence of conspecifics.

Moreover, various Birds modify their vigilance behaviours when they are in groups. Birds alone “regroup” their eye- blinks during feeding times, in order to better monitor their environment. Also, blink duration seems to be shorter when Birds are alone [218] and bigger the group is, longer is the time spent blinking.

Table 1: Comparison of facial muscles in various Mammals with their respective facial movements (the list is not exhaustive).

Muscles Species and references Associated Action Unit and Action Descriptor
Frontalis Chimpanzee [229]
Hylobatid [15]
Macaque [17]
Orangutan [230]
Cat [20]
Dog [18]
Horse [19]
Suidae [231]
Camel [232]
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Procerus Chimpanzee [229]
Hylobatid [15]
Macaque [17]
Orangutan [230]
Nd
41 (Glabella lowerer)
41
4 (Brow Lowerer)
Mentalis Chimpanzee [229]
Hylobatid [15]
Macaque [17]
Orangutan [230]
Cat [20]
Dog [18]
Horses [19]
Suidae [231]
Camel [232]
17 (Chin Raiser)
17
17
17
Nd
17, 16+17
17
Nd
Nd
Risorius Chimpanzee [229] Nd
Mouth    
Orbicularis oris Chimpanzee [229]
Hylobatid [15]
Macaque [17]
Orangutan [230]
Cat [20]
Dog [18]
Horses [19]
Rabbit [233]
Mice [234]
Rat [51]
Ferret [29]
Pig [235]
Sheep [236]
Camel [232]
Dolphin [85]
22 (Lip funeler), 24 (Lip pressor), 25 (Lips part), 28 (Lip suck)
18 (Lip pucker), 22, 25, 28
8 (Lip toward each other), 18, 25
18, 22, 24, 25, 28
118(Lip pucker), 25
118, 25,
118, 122 (Upper lip curler), 24, 25
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Levator labii superioris Chimpanzee [229]
Hylobatid [15]
Macaque [17]
Orangutan [230]
Cat [20]
Dog [18]
Horse [19]
Rabbit [233]
Mice [234]
Rat [51]
Pig [235]
Sheep [236]
Camel [232]
Dolphin [85]
10 (Upper lip raiser) 25 (Lips part)
10, 25
10, 25
10, 25
125 (Lips part)
110 (Upper lip raiser), 125
125
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Levator nasolabialis Cat [20]
Dog [18]
Horse [19]
Rabbit [237]
Mice [238]
Rat [51]
Ferret [29]
Pig [235]
Sheep [239]
Camel [232]
Seal [240]
Dolphin [85]
25, 109+110 (nose wrinkle+upper lip raiser)
10, 25, 109+110
10, 113 (Cheek puffer), 25, 109+110
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Caninus Chimpanzee [229]
Cat [20]
Dog [18]
Rat [51]
Sheep [239]
Camel [232]
13
25, 109+110
10, 25, 109+110
Nd
Nd
Nd
Depressor labii inferioris Chimpanzee [229]
Hylobatid [15]
Macaque [17]
Orangutan [230]
Cat [20]
Horse [19]
Rabbit [241]
Suidae [231]
16 (Lower lip depressor), 25
16, 25
16, 25
16, 25
16
16, 17 (chin raiser), 25
Nd
Nd
Depressor labii superioris Suidae [231]
Sheep [239]
Camel [239]
Nd
Nd
Nd
Depressor anguli oris Chimpanzee [229]
Hylobatid [15]
Macaque [17]
Orangutan [230]
Rabbit [237]
Mice [242]
Sheep [236]
15 (Lip corner depressor)
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Chimpanzee [229]
Levator anguli oris Chimpanzee [229]
Gibbon [15]
Macaque [17]
Orangutan [230]
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Buccinator Chimpanzee [229]
Hylobatid [15]
macaque [17]
Orangutan [230]
Cat [20]
Dog [18]
Rabbit [243]
Mice [244]
Rat [51]
Pig [235]
Sheep [245]
Camel [232]
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
118
116 (Lower lip depressor), 118
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Zygomaticus Chimpanzee [229]
Hylobatid [15]
Macaque [17]
Orangutan [230]
Cat [20]
Dog [18]
Horse [19]
Rabbit [237]
Rat [51]
Mice [234]
Ferret [29]
Sheep [236]
Camel [232]
Seal [30]
12 (Lip corner puller)
12
12
12
12
12
12, 25 (Lips part)
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Incisivii labii Hylobatid [15]
Macaque [17]
Orangutan [230]
Horse [19]
Camel [232]
18
18
18
18
Nd
Eyes    
Orbicularis oculi Chimpanzee [229]
Hylobatid [15]
Macaque [17]
Orangutan [230]
Cat [20]
Dog [18]
Horse [19]
Rabbit [246]
Mice [242]
Rat [51]
Ferret [29]
Suidae [231]
Sheep [236]
Camel [232]
Dolphin [85]
Seal [240]
6 (Cheek Raiser), 43 (Eye closure), 45(Blink)
5 (Upper Lid Raiser), 6, 7 (Lid Tightener), 43, 45
6, 8 (Lips toward), 43, 45
6, 43, 45
143 (Eye closure), 145 (Blink), 47 (Half-blink)
143, 145
145
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nose    
Levator labii superioris alaquae nasi Chimpanzee [229]
Gibbon [15]
Macaque [17]
Orangutan [230]
Cat [20]
Dog [18]
Horse [19]
Ferret [29]
Sheep [236]
Seal [30]
9 (Nose wrinkle)
9
9
9
109+110
109+110
10, 113 (Sharp lip puller), AUH 13 (Nostril lift)
Nose wrinkle
Nostril litf
Nostril lift
Whiskers    
Muscles described in cat: Lateralis nasi, orbicularis oris, caninus

Cat [20]


Ferret [29]
Seal [30]
Rat [22]
Mouse [21]

200 (whisker retractor), 201 (whisker protractor), 202 (whisker raiser)
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Ears    
Muscles described in macaque Auricularis anterior (EAU101), superior (EAU102) and posterior (EAU103)

Macaque [17]


Cat [20]


Dog [18]
Horse [19]

EAD/EAU 101 (ears forward), 102 (ears adductor), 103 (ears flattener)
EAD 101, 102, 103, 104 (ears rotator), 105 (ears downward), 106 (ears backward), 
107 (ears constrictor)
EAD 101, 102, 103, 104, 105
EAD 101, 102, 103, 104
Head   Action descriptor
 

Chimpanzee [229]


Cat [20]
Dog [18]
Horse [19]
Macaque [17]
Hylobatid [15]

51 (head turn left), 52 (head turn right), 53 (head up), 54 (head down), 55 (head tilt left), AD 56 (head tilt right), 57 (head forward), 58 (head back)
51, 52, 53, 54, 55, 56, 57, 58
51, 52, 53, 54, 55, 56
55, 56
Nd
Nd
Non cutaneous muscle    
Platysma Chimpanzee [229]
Hylobatid [15]
Macaque [17]
Orangutan [230]
Cat [20]
Dog [18]
Horse [19]
Rabbit [247]
Rat [51]
Ferret [29]
Mice [234]
Sheep [236]
Pig [248]
Seal [240]
Nd
21 (Neck tightener)
Nd
Nd
Nd
12, 25 (Lips part), 116 (Lower lip depressor)
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
 
Masseter Chimpanzee[229]
Hylobatid [15]
Macaque [17]
Orangutan [230]
Cat [20]
Dog [18]
Horse [19]
Rabbit [243]
Rat [249]
Mice [238]
Ferret [29]
Sheep [239]
Pig [248]
Camel [232]

Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
26, 27
26, (Jaw drop), 27 (Mouth stretch)
26, 27
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd

Pterygoid Chimpanzee [229]
Hylobatid [15]
Macaque [17]
Orangutan [230]
Cat [20]
Dog [18]
Horse [19]
Ferret [250]
Pig [248]
Rat [251]
Rabbit [252]
Mice [253]
Sheep [236]
Camel [254] [255]
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
26, 27
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Nd
Various non-cutaneous muscles Chimpanzee [256]
Hylobatid [256]
Macaque [256]
Orangutan [256]
26, 27
26, 27
26, 27
26, 27
Abbreviations: Nd = presence of the muscle in this species but no associated action is described in the literature; AU= Action Units, facial movements generated by specific muscle groups [257]; AD = Action Descriptors, facial movements without a clear identified muscular basis [257]; EAU= Ears Action Unit; EAD = Ears Action Descriptor.
ABBREVIATIONS

AU: Action Unit; AD: Action descriptor; FACS: Facial action coding system; CN: Cranial nerve

DISCUSSION

Facial expressions are currently cluster to Mammals, based on the exclusive presence of cutaneous muscles in this class. They reveal internal states such as emotions, which can serve as public information and social communicative tools [1,2]. Yet emotions are also suggested to be expressed by non-mammals, as reptiles and birds. Stress-related tachycardia may have emerged in the class of Amniotes, as frogs and fishes do not disclose such phenomena during human-manipulation [219–221]. Moreover, sensory pleasure -a cognitive experience- is suggested to have already emerged with reptiles (Iguana iguana) [222]. It is however unclear if non-Amniotes have emotions as fear. As emotions are also expressed by non-mammals’ Amniotes, we explore the concept of facial communication from an evolutionary point of view based on current knowledge and examine how far it might not only be performed by Mammals, but more broadly by Amniotes.

Both Mammals and non-mammals’ Amniotes express meaningful facial communicative movements, as the headbob in parrots and lizards, the erection of facial feathers in jays and parrots or the dewlap in Anolis lizards [161], [180], [181], [211]. Social factors seem to influence the development of complex facial signals, as it has been demonstrated in Mammals with the group size, but also in lizards with sexual competition for example [216]. Facial movements are also used to enhance group cohesion, which is well described in Mammals [94]. Various other factors as mating systems [121,122] or the social context, such as the “audience effect” [154,155], seem to influence facial communication both in Mammals and non-mammals, even though the first phenomenon has only been studied in Primates. So, social factors’ influence on facial movements is not clustered to Mammals. However, it appears that sociality cannot explain alone complex communication signals [216].

Facial expressions might have been shaped by ecological factors too. For example, visual acuity is correlated with facial expressions complexity in Primates [95–97,100]. Some lizards, using facial ornaments, display an improved visual communication due to their evolution to an arboreal lifestyle [216]. But as there is few information on this topic, these examples cannot be generalized. For example, even species with poor visual acuity performed facial expressions [102,103]. Nonetheless, facial expressions are considered as one of the most accurate visual communicative tools and should necessitate a sufficiently large visual acuity. Thus, more studies should investigate the relation between visual acuity and the diversity of facial movements.

Despite the paramount communicative function of facial movements that seems quite similar among Amniotes, their use relies on more practical basis, the facial muscles. It is well known that Mammals are the only clade to possess cutaneous facial muscles. Yet, cetaceans’ facial mask is almost entirely rigid [85]. So, an entire infraorder seems to lack facial expressions and characterizing facial communication to mammals seems unjustified. Furthermore, birds share the orbicularis oculi with mammals, implied in eye-blink [162]. Blinking is described as a facial expression in all FACS (AU45), but even if reptiles and birds do eye-blink too [108], [167], it is not mentioned up to now as a facial expression. Moreover, it bears other roles than physiological function and may have a communicative value at least in birds, as its frequency varied according to the audience [218]. Thus, it would be inconsistent to consider it differently in non-mammal species, at least in birds.

Some muscles involved in facial communication in non-mammals Amniotes have a common embryologic origin with the cutaneous muscles of the mammalians, as the ceratohyoid muscle in Anolis dewlap [182]. Considering the common origin with mammals’ cutaneous muscles and its use for visual communication, we suggest that dewlap might be considered as an equivalent of facial expression in non-Mammals. The fact that Mammals do not possess such ornament should not be an argument to exclude the dewlap movement of facial expressions category. It should be noted that each movement is not automatically shared by all species. For example, several facial expressions involving whiskers are only described in catFACS [107], even if other animals for which a FACS exists -as Primates-do not possess them. So, the exclusivity of a facial feature in one species should not be an argument to reject it as a valuable facial communication.

In the same way, to reject a facial communicative movement because they are produced by non-cutaneous muscles does not seem justified. Indeed, several masticator muscles -non-cutaneous- like the masseter are involved in facial action units described in FACS (AU26 Jaw drop) [15]–[21], which can lead to the “open-mouth” expression, well described in Primates’ social communication [58,129,134]. To select which species can perform facial communication based on the muscular innervation is not a justified argument too. Indeed, mammals’ cutaneous muscles are innervated by the CN VII [34] and the non-mammals’ muscles are innervated by other cranial nerve, such as the CN V [162,182,183]. But, as it has been developed earlier, muscles innervated by other nerves (as the masticatory ones) can bear a communicative value, and so in non-mammals’ Amniotes species too. Thus, facial communication should not be clustered to species with specific muscles or muscles’ innervation.

Another feature of a communicative role of facial muscles in Amniotes could be revealed by some neuroanatomical characteristics such as brain nuclei. Their size increases with the diversity of facial movements in Primates [67,68]. Similarly, in lizards, the number of motoneurons controlling the dewlap movements increases with the frequency of its use [191]. In birds, no such phenomenon has been described related to facial movements [189], yet, birds’ relative brain size is correlated with their level of sociality, as mammals’ one. These correlations between neuroanatomical structures, sociality and facial movements might suggest that facial movements have an important communicative value not only in Mammals but in Amniotes too. However, more studies on non-mammal species are needed to conclude about it.

Furthermore, it should be noted that despite anatomical differences, mutual comprehension of facial communicative and emotional displays exists in interspecies interactions. For example, primates as well as domesticated species as dogs and horses can discriminate humans’ emotions of joy or anger from facial movements [223–226]. This interspecies understanding provides a strong argument for an integrative evolutionary approach of the topic of facial behavior. Some studies are suggesting they also used other signals, as odors [227,228], so emotional recognition might be multimodal. The respective contribution of these items is not well understood and should also be further investigated. The ability to recognize humans’ emotions might be an important tool in human-animal relationship, including animal assisted therapies.

CONCLUSION

From this review, we propose that facial communication is not clustered to Mammals and to facial expressions. Evidence comes from the presence of movements with communicative value in non-Mammals, which do not express stricto sensu facial expressions as they have a rigid facial mask. We propose the following definitions: movements are visible displacements of body segments or tissues. They are motor actions that do not need cognitive and emotional implication, while behaviours require the interpretation of environmental or internal stimuli, as the presence of an audience or emotional experiences. Behaviours are movements that can be involved in social communication. So facial behaviours are not clustered to Mammals but might be largely expressed among Amniote’s species. Whereas facial expressions are facial behaviours generated by the contraction of cutaneous facial muscles, innervated by the CN VII, and thus only expressed by Mammals.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

All the authors participate equally to the redaction of the article and the literature analysis. We would like to thank Victoire Dejardin and Alex Garnaud for their help in the literature research and the proofreading. This work was carried out as part of the internship of Margaux Hallegot and Julian Lapique. The doctoral scholarship of Sophie Pellon was supported by the Prince Laurent Foundation.

DISCLOSURE

The authors disclaim that they have no financial interest or any conflict of interest for this review.

REFERENCES

1. Burrows AM. The facial expression musculature in primates and its evolutionary significance. Bioessays. 2008; 30: 212-225.

2. Parr LA. The evolution of face processing in primates. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. Biol Sci. 2011; 366: 1764-1777.

3. Ekman Group P. Homepage - Paul Ekman Group. Paul Ekman Group. 2020.

4. Tian Y-L, Kanade T, Cohn JF. Facial Expression Analysis. In: Li SZ, Jain AK, editors. Handbook of Face Recognition. New York: Springer; 2005; 247-275.

5. Harley JM. Chapter 5 - Measuring Emotions: A Survey of Cutting Edge Methodologies Used in Computer-Based Learning Environment Research. In: Tettegah SY, Gartmeier M, editors. Emotions, Technology, Design, and Learning. San Diego: Acad Press; 2016; 89-114.

6. Waller BM, Julle-Daniere E, Micheletta J. Measuring the evolution of facial ‘expression’ using multi-species FACS. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2020; 113: 1-11.

7. Davis DE. Integral animal behavior. 1966.

8. Barnett SA, Tinbergen N. The Study of Instinct. In: Clarendon Press, Oxford. Aggressive Behavior: Proceedings. 1969; 14.

9. Wallace AR, Sanders GP, Ferl RJ. Biology-The Science of Life. Pearson; 4 Edition. 1996.

10. Levitis DA, Lidicker WZ, Freund G. Behavioural biologists do not agree on what constitutes behaviour. Anim Behv. 2009; 78: 103-110.

11. Darwin C. L’expression des émotions chez l’homme et les animaux. Sn. 1877; 460.

12. Duchenne G-B. Mécanisme de la physionomie humaine: où, Analyse électro-physiologique de l’expression des passions. J.B. Baillière. 1876; 308.

13. Ekman P, Friesen WV. Constants across cultures in the face and emotion. J Person Soc Psychol. 1971; 17: 124-129.

14. Parr LA, Waller BM, Vick SJ, Bard KA. Classifying chimpanzee facial expressions using muscle action. Emotion. 2007; 7: 172-181.

15. Waller BM, Lembeck M, Kuchenbuch P, Burrows AM, Liebal K. GibbonFACS: A Muscle-Based Facial Movement Coding System for Hylobatids. Int J Primatol. 2012; 33: 809-821.

16. Caeiro CC, Waller BM, Zimmermann E, Burrows AM, Davila-Ross M. OrangFACS: A Muscle-Based Facial Movement Coding System for Orangutans (Pongo spp.). Int J Primatol. 2013; 34: 115-129.

17. Julle-Danière É, Micheletta J, Whitehouse J, Joly M, Gass C, Burrows AM, et al. MaqFACS (Macaque Facial Action Coding System) can be used to document facial movements in Barbary macaques ( Macaca sylvanus). PeerJ. 2015; 3: 1248.

18. Waller BM, Peirce K, Caeiro CC, Scheider L, Burrows AM, McCune S, et al. Paedomorphic Facial Expressions Give Dogs a Selective Advantage. Wade C, editor. PLoS ONE. 2013; 8: 82686.

19. Wathan J, Burrows AM, Waller BM, McComb K. EquiFACS: The Equine Facial Action Coding System. Hillmann E, editor. PLoS ONE. 2015; 10: e0131738.

20. Caeiro CC, Burrows AM, Waller BM. Development and application of CatFACS: Are human cat adopters influenced by cat facial expressions? Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 2017; 189: 66-78.

21. Langford DJ, Bailey AL, Chanda ML, Clarke SE, Drummond TE, Echols S, et al. Coding of facial expressions of pain in the laboratory mouse. Nat Methods. 2010; 7: 447-449.

22. Sotocina SG, Sorge RE, Zaloum A, Tuttle AH, Martin LJ, Wieskopf JS, et al. The Rat Grimace Scale: A Partially Automated Method for Quantifying Pain in the Laboratory Rat via Facial Expressions. Mol Pain. 2011; 7: 1744-8069-7-55.

23. Keating SCJ, Thomas AA, Flecknell PA, Leach MC. Evaluation of EMLA Cream for Preventing Pain during Tattooing of Rabbits: Changes in Physiological, Behavioural and Facial Expression Responses. Chapouthier G, editor. PLoS ONE. 2012; 7: 44437.

24. Dalla Costa E, Minero M, Lebelt D, Stucke D, Canali E, Leach MC. Development of the Horse Grimace Scale (HGS) as a Pain Assessment Tool in Horses Undergoing Routine Castration. Hillman E, editor. PLoS ONE. 2014; 9: 92281.

25. Guesgen MJ, Beausoleil NJ, Leach M, Minot EO, Stewart M, Stafford KJ. Coding and quantification of a facial expression for pain in lambs. Behav Proc. 2016; 132: 49-56.

26. McLennan KM, Rebelo CJB, Corke MJ, Holmes MA, Leach MC, Constantino-Casas F. Development of a facial expression scale using footrot and mastitis as models of pain in sheep. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2016; 176: 19-26.

27. Viscardi AV, Hunniford M, Lawlis P, Leach M, Turner PV. Development of a Piglet Grimace Scale to Evaluate Piglet Pain Using Facial Expressions Following Castration and Tail Docking: A Pilot Study. Front Vet Sci. 2017; 4.

28. Hempstead MN, Waas JR, Stewart M, Cave VM, Sutherland MA. Behavioural response of dairy goat kids to cautery disbudding. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2017; 194: 42-47.

29. Reijgwart ML, Schoemaker NJ, Pascuzzo R, Leach MC, Stodel M, de Nies L, et al. The composition and initial evaluation of a grimace scale in ferrets after surgical implantation of a telemetry probe. Staffieri F, editor. PLoS ONE. 2017; 12: 0187986.

30. MacRae AM, Joanna Makowska I, Fraser D. Initial evaluation of facial expressions and behaviours of harbour seal pups (Phoca vitulina) in response to tagging and microchipping. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2018; 205: 167-174.

31. Müller BR, Soriano VS, Bellio JCB, Molento CFM. Facial expression of pain in Nellore and crossbred beef cattle. Journal of Veterinary Behavior. 2019; 34: 60-65.

32. Evangelista MC, Watanabe R, Leung VSY, Monteiro BP, O’Toole E, Pang DSJ, et al. Facial expressions of pain in cats: the development and validation of a Feline Grimace Scale. Sci Rep. 2019; 9: 1-11.

33. Cabanac M, Cabanac AJ, Parent A. The emergence of consciousness in phylogeny. Behav Brain Res. 2009; 198: 267-272.

34. Barone R. Anatomie comparée des mammifères domestiques - Tome 2 - Arthrologie et Myologie - 4ème Edition. Vigot M,Editor. 2000; 1022.

35. Rinn WE. The neuropsychology of facial expression: A review of the neurological and psychological mechanisms for producing facial expressions. Psychol Bulletin. 1984; 95: 52-77.

36. Cobo JL, Abbate F, de Vicente JC, Cobo J, Vega JA. Searching for proprioceptors in human facial muscles. Neurosci Lett. 2017; 640: 1-5.

37. Stål P, Eriksson P-O, Eriksson A, Thornell L-E. Enzyme-histochemical differences in fibre-type between the human major and minor zygomatic and the first dorsal interosseus muscles. Arch Oral Biol. 1987; 32: 833-841.

38. Stål P, Eriksson P-O, Eriksson A, Thornell L-E. Enzyme-histochemical and morphological characteristics of muscle fibre types in the human buccinator and orbicularis oris. Arch Oral Biol. 1990; 35: 449-458.

39. Westbrook KE, Nessel TA, Varacallo M. Anatomy, Head and Neck, Facial Muscles. In: StatPearls. Treasure Island (FL): Stat Pearls Publishing; 2020. 

40. Goodmurphy CW, Ovalle WK. Morphological study of two human facial muscles: Orbicularis oculi and corrugator supercilii. Clin Anat. 1999; 12: 1-11.

41. Kaminski J, Waller BM, Diogo R, Hartstone-Rose A, Burrows AM. Evolution of facial muscle anatomy in dogs. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2019; 116: 14677-14681.

42. Yan W-J, Li X, Wang S-J, Zhao G, Liu Y-J, Chen Y-H, et al. CASME II: An Improved Spontaneous Micro-Expression Database and the Baseline Evaluation. Guo K, editor. PLoS ONE. 2014; 9: e86041.

43. Warren G, Schertler E, Bull P. Detecting Deception from Emotional and Unemotional Cues. J Nonverbal Behav. 2009; 33: 59-69.

44. Graham A, Poopalasundaram S, Shone V, Kiecker C. A reappraisal and revision of the numbering of the pharyngeal arches. J Anat. 2019; 235: 1019–1023.

45. KARDONG K. VERTEBRATES: comparative anatomy, function,evolution. New York: McGraw-Hill; 2006.

46. Sperber GH, Sperber SM, Guttmann GD. Craniofacial Embryogenetics and Development. PMPH-USA; 2010; 264.

47. Larsen WJ. Human embryology. Churchill Livingstone. 2001.

48. Dartt DA, Bex P, D’Amore P, Dana R, Mcloon L, Niederkorn J. Ocular Periphery and Disorders . Acad Press. 2011; 568.

49. Fransson B, Kippenes H, Silver GE, Gavin PR. Magnetic Resonance Diagnosis: Cavernous Sinus Syndrome in a Dog. Vet Radiol Ultrasound. 2000; 41: 536-538.

50. Peterson BW, Richmond FJ. Control of Head Movement. Oxford Uni Press. 1988; 348.

51. Diogo R, Wood BA, Aziz MA, Burrows A. On the origin, homologies and evolution of primate facial muscles, with a particular focus on hominoids and a suggested unifying nomenclature for the facial muscles of the Mammalia. J Anat. 2009; 215: 300-319.

52. Ekman P, Keltner D. Universal facial expression of emotion. California mental health. 1970; 8: 151.

53. Diogo R, Powell V. The Origin and Evolution of Mammalian Head Muscles with Special Emphasis on the Facial Myology of Primates and Modern Humans. In: Ziermann JM, Diaz Jr RE, Diogo R, editors. Heads, Jaws, and Muscles: Anatomical, Functional, and Developmental Diversity in Chordate Evolution. Cham: Springer Int Publ; 2019; 253- 293.

54. Peñaherrera Aguirre M, Woodley of Menie MA, Fernandes HBF. Relative Brain Size, Encephalization Quotient. In: Shackelford TK, Weekes-Shackelford VA, editors. Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science. Cham: Springer Int Publ. 2017; 1-3.

55. Kamilar JM, Cooper N. Phylogenetic signal in primate behaviour, ecology and life history. Phil Trans R Soc B. 2013; 368: 20120341.

56. Diogo R, Wood B. The broader evolutionary lessons to be learned from a comparative and phylogenetic analysis of primate muscle morphology. Biol Rev. 2013; 88: 988-1001.

57. Florkiewicz B, Skollar G, Reichard UH. Facial expressions and pair bonds in hylobatids. Am J Phys Anthropol . 2018; 167: 108-123.

58. Parr LA, Waller BM, Vick SJ. New Developments in Understanding Emotional Facial Signals in Chimpanzees. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2007; 16: 117-222.

59. Scheider L, Liebal K, Oña L, Burrows A, Waller B. A comparison of facial expression properties in five hylobatid species: Facial Expressions in Hylobatids. Am J Primatol. 2014; 76: 618-628.

60. Matano S. A Volumetric Comparison of the Vestibular Nuclei in Primates. FPR. 1986; 47: 189-203.

61. Baron G, Frahm HD, Stephan H. Comparison of brain structure volumes in insectivora and primates. VIII. Vestibular complex. J Hirnforsch. 1988; 29: 509-523.

62. Baron G, Stephan H, Frahm HD. Comparison of brain structure volumes in Insectivora and primates. IX. Trigeminal complex. J Hirnforsch. 1990; 31: 193-200.

63. Glendenning KK, Masterton RB. Comparative Morphometry of Mammalian Central Auditory Systems: Variation in Nuclei and Form of the Ascending System. BBE. 1998; 51: 59-89.

64. Hutcheon JM, Kirsch JAW, Jr TG. A Comparative Analysis of Brain Size in Relation to Foraging Ecology and Phylogeny in the Chiroptera. BBE. 2002; 60: 165-80.

65. Barton RA. Neocortex size and behavioural ecology in primates. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B. Biol Sci. 1996; 263: 173-177.

66. Johnson MA, Wade J. Behavioural display systems across nine Anolis lizard species: sexual dimorphisms in structure and function. Proc R Soc B. 2010; 277: 1711-1719.

67. Diogo R, Santana JA. Evolution of facial musculature. In: The Science of Facial Expression. Oxford Uni Press. 2017.

68. Dobson SD. Allometry of facial mobility in anthropoid primates: Implications for the evolution of facial expression. Am J Phys Anthropol. 2009; 138: 70-81.

69. Sherwood CC, Hof PR, Holloway RL, Semendeferi K, Gannon PJ, Frahm HD, et al. Evolution of the brainstem orofacial motor system in primates: a comparative study of trigeminal, facial, and hypoglossal nuclei. J Hum Evol. 2005; 48: 45-84.

70. Walberg F. Do the motor nuclei of the cranial nerves receive corticofugal fibers? An experimental study in the cat. Brain. 1957; 597-605.

71. Kuypers HGJM. An anatomical analysis of cortico-bulbar connexions to the pons and lower brain stem in the cat. J Anat. 1958; 92: 198-218.

72. Kuypers HGJM. Some projections from the peri-central cortex to the pons and lower brain stem in monkey and chimpanzee. J Comp Neurol. 1958; 110: 221-255.

73. Kuypers HGJM. The Anatomical Organization of the Descending Pathways and their Contributions to Motor Control Especially in Primates. Human Reflexes, Pathophysiology of Motor Systems, Methodology of Human Reflexes. 1973; 3: 38-68.

74. Jenny AB, Saper CB. Organization of the facial nucleus and corticofacial projection in the monkey a reconsideration of the upper motor neuron facial palsy. Neurology. 1987; 37: 930-930.

75. Sokoloff AJ. The organization of the hypoglossal nucleus: An experimental neuroanatomical investigation of hypoglossal-lingual and cortico-hypoglossal projections in Macaca fascicularis and other vertebrate species. 1990; 1.

76. Sokoloff A, Deacon TW. Direct projections from the face area of primary motor cortex to the facial nucleus in cynomolgus monkey but not in the cat or rat. Am J Phys Anthropol. 1990; 81.

77. Morecraft RJ, Louie JL, Herrick JL, Stilwell-Morecraft KS. Cortical innervation of the facial nucleus in the non-human primateA new interpretation of the effects of stroke and related subtotal brain trauma on the muscles of facial expression. Brain. 2001; 124: 176-208.

78. Jürgens U, Alipour M. A comparative study on the cortico-hypoglossal connections in primates, using biotin dextranamine. Neurosci Lett. 2002; 328: 245-248.

79. Travers JB, Norgren R. Afferent projections to the oral motor nuclei in the rat. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1983; 220: 280-298.

80. Fay RA, Norgren R. Identification of rat brainstem multisynaptic connections to the oral motor nuclei using pseudorabies virus. III. Lingual muscle motor systems. Brain Res Rev. 1997; 291-311.

81. Zeller AC. Communication by sight and smell. In: Smuts BB, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Wrangham RW, editors. Primate Societies. Uni Chicago Press. 1987.

82. Huber E. Evolution of Facial Musculature and Cutaneous Field of Trigeminus. Part I. Quar Rev Biol. 1930; 5: 133-88.

83. Graur D, Higgins DG. Molecular evidence for the inclusion of cetaceans within the order Artiodactyla. Mol Biol Evol. 1994; 11: 357-364.

84. Mass AM, Supin AYA. Adaptive features of aquatic mammals’ eye. Anat Rec. 2007; 290: 701-715.

85. Cozzi B, Huggenberger S, Oelschläger HA. Anatomy of Dolphins: Insights into Body Structure and Function. Acad Press. 2016; 458.

86. Hosokawa K. Further research on transplantation of Yoshida sarcoma with a single cell. II. Gan. 1951; 42: 343-345.

87. Butler AB, Hodos W. Comparative Vertebrate Neuroanatomy: Evolution and Adaptation. John Wiley Sons. 2005; 740.

88. Forêt R. Dictionnaire des sciences de la vie. De Boeck Superieur. 2018; 1299.

89. Meshida K, Lin S, Domning DP, Reidenberg JS, Wang P, Gilland E. Cetacean Orbital Muscles: Anatomy and Function of the Circular Layers. Anat Rec. 2019; 24278.

90. O’corry-Crowe GM. Beluga Whale: Delphinapterus leucas. In: Perrin WF, Würsig B, Thewissen JGM, editors. Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals (Second Edition). London: Acad Press; 2009; 108-112.

91. Harper CJ, McLellan WA, Rommel SA, Gay DM, Dillaman RM, Pabst DA. Morphology of the melon and its tendinous connections to the facial muscles in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). J Morphol. 2008; 269: 820-839.

92. Mukherjee A, Mukherjee PK. Spectacular Snow-White Beluga Whale. Sci rep. 2018; 2.

93. Overstrom NA. Association between burst-pulse sounds and aggressive behavior in captive Atlantic bottlenosed dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Zoo Biol. 1983; 2: 93-103.

94. Dobson SD. Socioecological correlates of facial mobility in nonhuman anthropoids. Am J Phys Anthropol. 2009; 139: 413-420.

95. Howland HC, Merola S, Basarab JR. The allometry and scaling of the size of vertebrate eyes. Vision Res. 2004; 44: 2043-2065.

96. Kiltie RA. Scaling of visual acuity with body size in mammals and birds. Funct Ecology. 2000; 14: 226-234.

97. Veilleux CC, Kirk EC. Visual Acuity in Mammals: Effects of Eye Size and Ecology. Brain Behav Evol. 2014; 83: 43-53.

98. Ross CF. Into the Light: The Origin of Anthropoidea. Annu Rev Anthropol. 2000; 29: 147-194.

99. Kay RF, Kirk EC. Osteological evidence for the evolution of activity pattern and visual acuity in primates. Am J Phys Anthropol. 2000; 113: 235-262.

100. Tate AJ, Fischer H, Leigh AE, Kendrick KM. Behavioural and neurophysiological evidence for face identity and face emotion processing in animals. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. Biol Sci. 2006; 361: 2155-2172.

101. Norberg, Rayner. Ecological morphology and flight in bats (Mammalia; Chiroptera): wing adaptations, flight performance, foraging strategy and echolocation. Phil Trans R Soc Lond B. 1987; 316: 335-427.

102. Prusky GT, West PWR, Douglas RM. Behavioral assessment of visual acuity in mice and rats. Vision Research. 2000; 40: 2201-2209.

103. Rehkämper G, Perrey A, Werner CW, Opfermann-Rüngeler C, Görlach A. Visual perception and stimulus orientation in cattle. Vision Res. 2000; 40: 2489-2497.

104. Mass AM, Supin AY. Retinal Topography and Visual Acuity in the Riverine Tucuxi (sotalia Fluviatilis). Marine Mamm Sci. 1999; 15: 351-365.

105. Tada H, Omori Y, Hirokawa K, Ohira H, Tomonaga M. Eye-Blink Behaviors in 71 Species of Primates. PLoS One. 2013; 8.

106. Kirsten SJ, Kirsten EB. Spontaneous Blink Rates of Birds. The Condor. 1983; 85: 92-93.

107. Correia Caeiro C, Waller B, Burrows A. The Cat Facial Action Coding System manual (CatFACS). Uni Portsmouth. 2013.

108. Gy W, Ja S, Jw S. A comparative study of the tapetum, retina and skull of the ferret, dog and cat. Lab Anim Sci. 1985; 35: 200-210.

109. Wathan J, Proops L, Grounds K, McComb K. Horses discriminate between facial expressions of conspecifics. Sci Rep. 2016; 6: 38322.

110. Nakashima SF, Ukezono M, Nishida H, Sudo R, Takano Y. Receiving of emotional signal of pain from conspecifics in laboratory rats. R Soc opens sci. 2015; 2: 140381.

111. Morimoto Y, Fujita K. Capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) modify their own behaviors according to a conspecific’s emotional expressions. Primates. 2011; 52: 279-286.

112. Palagi E, Marchi E, Cavicchio P, Bandoli F. Sharing playful mood: rapid facial mimicry in Suricata suricatta. Anim Cogn. 2019; 22: 719- 732.

113. GOODALL J. The chimpanzees of Gombe?: patterns of behavior. Cambridge Mass. 1986.

114. Bartlett TQ. The Gibbons of Khao Yai: Seasonal Variation in Behavior and Ecology, CourseSmart eTextbook. Routledge. 2015; 193.

115. Vick S-J, Waller BM, Parr LA, Smith Pasqualini MC, Bard KA. A Cross-species Comparison of Facial Morphology and Movement in Humans and Chimpanzees Using the Facial Action Coding System (FACS). J Nonverbal Behav. 2007; 31: 1-20.

116. Preuschoft S, van Hooff JARAM. Homologizing Primate Facial Displays: A Critical Review of Methods. FPR. 1995; 65: 121-137.

117. Preuschoft S, van Hooff JARAM. The social function of ‘smile’ and ‘laughter’: Variations across primate species and societies. Hillsdale, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Inc. 1997; 171.

118. Fuentes A. Hylobatid communities: Changing views on pair bonding and social organization in hominoids. Am J Phy Anthropol. 2000; 113: 33-60.

119. Burrows AM, Li L, Waller BM, Micheletta J. Social variables exert selective pressures in the evolution and form of primate mimetic musculature. J Anat. 2016; 228: 595-607.

120. Fleagle JG. Primate Adaptation and Evolution. Acad Press. 2013; 495.

121. Burrows AM, Diogo R, Waller BM, Bonar CJ, Liebal K. Evolution of the Muscles of Facial Expression in a Monogamous Ape: Evaluating the Relative Influences of Ecological and Phylogenetic Factors in Hylobatids. Anat Record. 2011; 294: 645-663.

122. Diogo R, Potau JM, Pastor JF, dePaz F, Ferrero EM, Bello G, et al. Photographic and Descriptive Musculoskeletal Atlas of Gibbons and Siamangs (Hylobates) . CRC Press, 1st Edition. 2012; 160.

123. Taylor D, Hartmann D, Dezecache G, Te Wong S, Davila-Ross M. Facial Complexity in Sun Bears: Exact Facial Mimicry and Social Sensitivity. Sci Rep. 2019; 9: 1-6.

124. Goren D, Wilson HR. Quantifying facial expression recognition across viewing conditions. Vision Res. 2006; 46: 1253-1262.

125. Lazow SP, Bergman TJ. The structural and motivational role of the unique lip?flip movement in the gelada (Theropithecus gelada) facial display repertoire. Am J Phys Anthropol. 2020; 24031.

126. Ellis SL. Recognising and assessing feline emotions during the consultation: History, body language and behaviour. J Feline Med Surg. 2018; 20: 445-456.

127. Trösch M, Pellon S, Cuzol F, Parias C, Nowak R, Calandreau L, et al. Horses feel emotions when they watch positive and negative horse– human interactions in a video and transpose what they saw to real life. Anim Cogn. 2020;

128. Finka LR, Luna SP, Brondani JT, Tzimiropoulos Y, McDonagh J, Farnworth MJ, et al. Geometric morphometrics for the study of facial expressions in non-human animals, using the domestic cat as an exemplar. Sci Rep. 2019; 9: 1-12.

129. Pritsch C, Telkemeyer S, Mühlenbeck C, Liebal K. Perception of facial expressions reveals selective affect-biased attention in humans and orangutans. Sci Rep. 2017; 7: 7782.

130. Kirkevold BC, Lockard JS, Heestand JE. Development comparisons of grimace and play mouth in infant pigtail macaques (Macaca nemestrina). Am J Primatol. 1982; 3: 277-283.

131. Meridda A, Gazzano A, Mariti C. Assessment of dog facial mimicry: Proposal for an emotional dog facial action coding system (EMDOGFACS). 2014; 9: 3.

132. Hook-costigan MA, Rogers LJ. Lateralized use of the mouth in production of vocalizations by marmosets. Neuropsychologia. 1998; 36: 1265-1273.

133. Chevalier-Skolnikoff S. Visual and tactile communication inMacaca arctoides and its ontogenetic development. Am J Phys Anthropol. 1973; 38: 515-518.

134. Preuschoft S. “Laughter” and “Smile” in Barbary Macaques (Macaca sylvanus). Ethology. 2010; 91: 220-236.

135. Schneider C, Call J, Liebal K. Do bonobos say NO by shaking their head? Primates. 2010; 51: 199-202.

136. MacRae AM. Facial expression, vocalizations and eye temperature as potential indicators of pain in harbour seals (Phoca vitulina). [Vancouver]: Uni British Columbia. 2018.

137. Di Giminiani P, Brierley VLMH, Scollo A, Gottardo F, Malcolm EM, Edwards SA, et al. The Assessment of Facial Expressions in Piglets Undergoing Tail Docking and Castration: Toward the Development of the Piglet Grimace Scale. Front Vet Sci. 2016; 3.

138. Gleerup KB, Forkman B, Lindegaard C, Andersen PH. An equine pain face. Vet Anaes Analg. 2015; 42: 103-114.

139. Andrew RJ. The Origins of Facial Expressions. Sci Am. 1965; 213: 88- 94.

140. Jack RE, Garrod OGB, Schyns PG. Dynamic Facial Expressions of Emotion Transmit an Evolving Hierarchy of Signals over Time. Curr Biol. 2014; 24: 187-192.

141. Marsh AA, Adams RB, Kleck RE. Why Do Fear and Anger Look the Way They Do? Form and Social Function in Facial Expressions. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2005; 31: 73-86. 

142. Waller BM, Dunbar RIM. Differential Behavioural Effects of Silent Bared Teeth Display and Relaxed Open Mouth Display in Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Ethology. 2005; 111: 129-142.

143. Van Hooff JARAM. The Facial Displays of the Catarrhine Monkeys and Apes. In: Primate ethology. New Brunswick, NJ, US: AldineTransaction; 1967; 7-68.

144. Davila-Ross M, Jesus G, Osborne J, Bard KA. Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) Produce the Same Types of ‘Laugh Faces’ when They Emit Laughter and when They Are Silent. PLoS One. 2015; 10.

145. Fox MW. A Comparative Study of the Development of Facial Expressions in Canids; Wolf, Coyote and Foxes. Behav. 1970; 36: 49- 73.

146. Ueno A, Ueno Y, Tomonaga M. Facial responses to four basic tastes in newborn rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Behav Brain Res 2004; 154: 261-271.

147. Bråten S, Br°aten S, Oatley PED of HD& APK, Manstead A, Oatley K. Intersubjective Communication and Emotion in Early Ontogeny. Cambridge University Press. 1998; 486.

148. Steiner JE. Human Facial Expressions in Response to Taste and Smell Stimulation. In: Reese HW, Lipsitt LP, editors. Adv Child Dev Behav. JAI. 1979; 257-295.

149. Vanderwert RE, Simpson EA, Paukner A, Suomi SJ, Fox NA, Ferrari PF. Early Social Experience Affects Neural Activity to Affiliative Facial Gestures in Newborn Nonhuman Primates. DNE. 2015; 37: 243-252.

150. Hamilton AF de C, Lind F. Audience effects: what can they tell us about social neuroscience, theory of mind and autism? Cult Brain. 2016; 4: 159-177.

151. Jones SS, Collins K, Hong H-W. An Audience Effect on Smile Production in 10-Month-Old Infants. Psychol Sci. 1991; 2: 45-49.

152. Schmidt KL, Cohn JF. Human facial expressions as adaptations: Evolutionary questions in facial expression research. Am J Phys Anthropol. 2001; 116: 3-24.

153. Wagner HL, Smith J. Facial expression in the presence of friends and strangers. J Nonverbal Behav. 1991; 15: 201-214.

154. Scheider L, Waller BM, Oña L, Burrows AM, Liebal K. Social Use of Facial Expressions in Hylobatids. Zimmermann E, editor. PLoS ONE. 2016; 11: e0151733.

155. Waller BM, Caeiro CC, Davila-Ross M. Orangutans modify facial displays depending on recipient attention. Peer J. 2015; 3: 827.

156. Kaminski J, Hynds J, Morris P, Waller BM. Human attention affects facial expressions in domestic dogs. Sci Rep. 2017; 7: 1-7.

157. Price J, Catriona S, Welsh EM, Waran NK. Preliminary evaluation of a behaviour–based system for assessment of post–operative pain in horses following arthroscopic surgery. Vet Anaesth Analg. 2003; 30: 124-37.

158. Proops L, McComb K. Attributing attention: the use of human-given cues by domestic horses (Equus caballus). Anim Cogn. 2010; 13: 197-205.

159. Proops L, McComb K, Reby D. Cross-modal individual recognition in domestic horses (Equus caballus ). PNAS. 2009; 106: 947-951.

160. Tomberg C, Petit M, Pellon S. Horses display facial microexpressions. 2020.

161. Bertin A, Beraud A, Lansade L, Blache M-C, Diot A, Mulot B, et al. Facial display and blushing: Means of visual communication in blue-and-yellow macaws (Ara Ararauna)? PLoS One. 2018; 13. 

162. Jones MP, Pierce KE, Ward D. Avian Vision: A Review of Form and Function with Special Consideration to Birds of Prey. Journal of Exotic Pet Medicine. 2007; 16: 69-87.

163. Guerra-Fuentes RA, Roscito JG, Nunes PMS, Oliveira-Bastos PR, Antoniazzi MM, Carlos J, et al. Through the Looking Glass: The Spectacle in Gymnophthalmid Lizards: The Spectacle in Gymnophthalmidae. Anat Rec. 2014; 297: 496-504.

164. Zhu D, Keifer J. Pathways Controlling Trigeminal and Auditory Nerve-Evoked Abducens Eyeblink Reflexes in Pond Turtles. BBE. 2004; 64: 207-222.

165. Gröning F, Jones MEH, Curtis N, Herrel A, O’Higgins P, Evans SE, et al. The importance of accurate muscle modelling for biomechanical analyses: a case study with a lizard skull. J R Soc Interface. 2013; 10: 20130216.

166. Cundall D. Activity of Head Muscles during Feeding by Snakes: A Comparative Study. Am Zool. 1983; 23: 383-396.

167. Abdala V, Moro S. A cladistic analysis of ten lizard families (Reptilia: squamata) based on cranial musculature. Russ J Herpetol. 2003; 1: 53-78.

168. Johnston P. Homology of the Jaw Muscles in Lizards and Snakes-A Solution from a Comparative Gnathostome Approach: Snake Jaw Adductors. Anat Rec. 2014; 297: 574-85.

169. Bhattacharyya BN. Avian Jaw Function: Adaptation of the Seven– Muscle System and a Review. Proc Zool Soc. 2013; 66: 75-85.

170. Dubbeldam JL. Brainstem Mechanisms for Feeding in Birds: Interaction or Plasticity. BBE. 1984; 25: 85-98.

171. Goodwin D. Further Observations on the Behaviour of the Jay Garrulus Glandarius. Ibis. 1956; 98: 186-219.

172. Jouventin P, Dobson FS. Why Penguins Communicate: The Evolution of Visual and Vocal Signals. Acad Press. 2017; 334.

173. Zhang J-H, Tang W, Zhang Z-X, Luan B-Y, Yu S-B, Sui H-J. Connection of the Posterior Occipital Muscle and Dura Mater of the Siamese Crocodile: “Myodural Bridge” Structure in Siamese Crocodiles. Anat Rec. 2016; 299: 1402-1408.

174. Tsuihiji T, Kearney M, Rieppel O. Finding the neck-trunk boundary in snakes: Anteroposterior dissociation of myological characteristics in snakes and its implications for their neck and trunk body regionalization. J Morphol. 2012; 273: 992-1009.

175. Herrel A, Cleuren J, De Vree F. Prey capture in the lizardAgama stellio. J Morphol. 1995; 224: 313-29.

176. Kuroda N. On the cervical muscles of birds. J-STAGE. 1962; 3: 189- 211.

177. Shine R. Function and evolution of the frill of the frillneck lizard, Chlamydosaurus kingii (Sauria: Agamidae). Biol J Linn Soc. 1990; 40: 11-20.

178. Ord TJ, Martins EP. Tracing the origins of signal diversity in anole lizards: phylogenetic approaches to inferring the evolution of complex behavior. Ani Behav. 2006; 71: 1411-1429.

179. Ferguson GW. Display and Communications in Reptiles: An Historical Perspective. Am Zool. 1977; 17: 167-176.

180. Nicholson KE, Harmon LJ, Losos JB. Evolution of Anolis Lizard Dewlap Diversity. Tregenza T, editor. PLoS ONE. 2007; 2: 274.

181. Fitzpatrick S. Colour schemes for birds: structural coloration and signals of quality in feathers. Annales Zoologici Fennici. 1998; 35: 67-77

182. Romer AS, Parsons TS. The vertebrate body. 5th Edn. Philadelphia: Saunders. 1977; 624.

183. Tong J, Lopez MJ, Patel BC. Anatomy, Head and Neck, Eye Orbicularis Oculi Muscle. In: StatPearls . Treasure Island (FL): Stat Pearls Publ. 2020.

184. Daza JD, Diogo R, Johnston P, Abdala V. Jaw Adductor Muscles across Lepidosaurs: A Reappraisal. Anat Rec. 2011; 294: 1765-1782.

185. Jones MEH, Curtis N, O’Higgins P, Fagan M, Evans SE. The head and neck muscles associated with feeding in sphenodon (Reptilia: Lepidosauria: Rhynchocephalia). Palaeontologia Electronica. 2009; 12: 56.

186. Schrödl F, Brehmer A, Neuhuber WL, Nickla D. The Autonomic Facial Nerve Pathway in Birds: A Tracing Study in Chickens. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2006; 47: 3225.

187. Székely T, Catchpole CK, Devoodg A, Marchl Z, Devoogd TJ. Evolutionary changes in a song control area of the brain (HVC) are associated with evolutionary changes in song repertoire among European warblers (Sylviidae). Proc R Soc B. 1996; 263: 607-610.

188. Devoogd TJ, Krebs JR, Healy SD, Purvis A. Relations between song repertoire size and the volume of brain nuclei related to song: comparative evolutionary analyses amongst oscine birds. Proc Royal Soc B. 1993; 254: 75-82.

189. Shultz S, Dunbar RIM. Social bonds in birds are associated with brain size and contingent on the correlated evolution of life-history and increased parental investment: AVIAN BRAIN EVOLUTION. Biol J Linn Soc. 2010; 100: 111-123.

190. Wyles JS, Kunkel JG, Wilson AC. Birds, behavior, and anatomical evolution. PNAS. 1983; 80: 4394-4397.

191. Wade J. Sexual Dimorphisms in the Brainstem of the Green Anole Lizard. Brain Behav Evol. 1998; 52: 46-54.

192. Fernandez-Juricic E. Flock density, social foraging, and scanning: an experiment with starlings. Behav Ecol. 2004; 15: 371-379.

193. Reymond L. Spatial visual acuity of the eagle Aquila audax: a behavioural, optical and anatomical investigation. Vision Res. 1985; 25: 1477-1491.

194. Reymond L. Spatial visual acuity of the falcon, Falco berigora: A behavioural, optical and anatomical investigation. Vision Res. 1987; 27: 1859-1874.

195. Potier S, Bonadonna F, Kelber A, Martin GR, Isard P-F, Dulaurent T, et al. Visual abilities in two raptors with different ecology. J Exp Biol. 2016; 219: 2639-2649.

196. Blackwell BF, Fernández-Juricic E, Seamans TW, Dolan T. Avian visual system configuration and behavioural response to object approach. Anim Behav. 2009; 77: 673-684.

197. Fleishman LJ, Yeo AI, Perez CW. Visual acuity and signal color pattern in an Anolis lizard. J Exp Biol. 2017; 220: 2154-2158.

198. Baker RA, Gawne TJ, Loop MS, Pullman S. Visual acuity of the midland banded water snake estimated from evoked telencephalic potentials. J Comp Physiol A. 2007; 193: 865-870.

199. Ehrenfeld DW, Koch AL. Visual Accommodation in the Green Turtle. Sci. 1967; 155: 827-828.

200. Yorzinski JL. Eye blinking in an avian species is associated with gaze shifts. Sci Rep. 2016; 6: 32471.

201. Ward MP, Alessi M, Benson TJ, Chiavacci SJ. The active nightlife of diurnal birds: extraterritorial forays and nocturnal activity patterns. Anim Behav. 2014; 88: 175-184.

202. Turpie JK, Hockey P a. R. Comparative diurnal and nocturnal foraging behaviour and energy intake of premigratory Grey Plovers Pluvialis squatarola and Whimbrels Numenius phaeopus in South Africa. Ibis. 1993; 135: 156-165.

203. Davenport J. Environmental Stress and Behavioural Adaptation. Springer Sci Bus Media. 2012; 128.

204. Rand. Ecology and Social Organization in the lguanid Lizard Anolis lineatopus. Proceedings of the United States National Museum. 1967; 122: 79.

205. Sampaio PRM. First report of predation by a caiman (Paleosuchus trigonatus, Crocodylia: Alligatoridae) on a caecilian (Caecilia marcusi, Gymnophiona: Caecilidae). 2013; 2.

206. Tsai T-S, Tu M-C. Postprandial thermophily of Chinese green tree vipers, Trimeresurus s. stejnegeri: Interfering factors on snake temperature selection in a thigmothermal gradient. J Therm Biol. 2005; 30: 423-430.

207. Raxworthy CJ, Pearson RG, Zimkus BM, Reddy S, Deo AJ, Nussbaum RA, et al. Continental speciation in the tropics: contrasting biogeographic patterns of divergence in the Uroplatus leaf-tailed gecko radiation of Madagascar. J Zool. 2008; 275: 423-440.

208. Péron F, Rat-Fischer L, Nagle L, Bovet D. ‘Unwilling’ versus ‘unable’: Do grey parrots understand human intentional actions? IS. 2010; 11: 428-441.

209. Spoon TR, Millam JR, Owings DH. Behavioural compatibility, extrapair copulation and mate switching in a socially monogamous parrot. Anim Behav. 2007; 73: 815-824.

210. Morris D. The Feather Postures of Birds and the Problem of the Origin of Social Signals. Behav. 1956; 9: 75-113.

211. Smith GA. Systematics of Parrots. Ibis. 1975; 117: 18-68.

212. Steffen JE, Guyer CC. Display behaviour and dewlap colour as predictors of contest success in brown anoles: Dewlap Colour and Behaviour in Contests. Biol J Linn Soc Lond. 2014; 111: 646-655.

213. Harrison A. Size-Assortative Pairing and Social Monogamy in a Neotropical Lizard, Anolis limifrons (Squamata: Polychrotidae). brvo. 2013; 534: 1-9.

214. Cezilly F, Nager RG. Comparative evidence for a positive association between divorce and extra-pair paternity in birds. Proc Royal Soc B Lon Series Biol Sci. 1995; 262: 7-12.

215. Arnqvist G, Kirkpatrick M. The Evolution of Infidelity in Socially Monogamous Passerines: The Strength of Direct and Indirect Selection on Extrapair Copulation Behavior in Females. Am Naturalist. 2005; 165: S26-37.

216. Ord TJ, Garcia-Porta J. Is sociality required for the evolution of communicative complexity? Evidence weighed against alternative hypotheses in diverse taxonomic groups. Phil Trans R Soc B. 2012; 367: 1811-1828.

217. Shaw RC, Clayton NS. Eurasian jays, Garrulus glandarius, flexibly switch caching and pilfering tactics in response to social context. Ani Behav. 2012; 84: 1191-1200.

218. Beauchamp G. Half-Blind to the Risk of Predation. Front Ecol Evol. 2017; 5.

219. Cabanac A, Cabanac M. Heart rate response to gentle handling of frog and lizard. Behav Proc. 2000; 52: 89-95.

220. Cabanac M, Aizawa S. Fever and tachycardia in a bird (Gallus domesticus) after simple handling. Physiol Behav. 2000; 69: 541- 545.

221. Cabanac M, Laberge F. Fever in Goldfish Is Induced by Pyrogens But Not by Handling. Physiol Behav. 1998; 63: 377-379.

222. Balasko M, Cabanac M. Behavior of Juvenile Lizards (Iguana iguana) in a Conflict between Temperature Regulation and Palatable Food. Brain Behav Evol. 1998; 52: 257-62.

223. Müller CA, Schmitt K, Barber ALA, Huber L. Dogs Can Discriminate Emotional Expressions of Human Faces. CurrBiol. 2015; 25: 601- 605.

224. Albuquerque N, Guo K, Wilkinson A, Savalli C, Otta E, Mills D. Dogs recognize dog and human emotions. Biol Lett. 2016;12: 20150883.

225. Kanazawa S. Recognition of facial expressions in a Japanese monkey (Macaca fuscata) and humans (Homo sapiens). Primates. 1996; 37: 25-38.

226. Proops L, Grounds K, Smith AV, McComb K. Animals Remember Previous Facial Expressions that Specific Humans Have Exhibited. Curr Biol. 2018; 28: 1428-1432.

227. Siniscalchi M, d’Ingeo S, Quaranta A. The dog nose “KNOWS” fear: Asymmetric nostril use during sniffing at canine and human emotional stimuli. Behav Brain Res. 2016; 304: 34-41.

228. Sabiniewicz A, Tarnowska K, ?wi?tek R, Sorokowski P, Laska M. Olfactory-based interspecific recognition of human emotions: Horses (Equus ferus caballus) can recognize fear and happiness body odour from humans (Homo sapiens). Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2020; 230: 105072.

229. Vick S-J, Waller B, Parr L, Smith-Pasqualini M, Bard K. The Chimpanzee Facial Action Coding System. 2006; 75.

230. Caeiro CC, Waller BM, Zimmermann E, Burrows AM, Davila-Ross M. OrangFACS: A Muscle-Based Facial Movement Coding System for Orangutans (Pongo spp.). Int J Primatol. 2013; 34: 115-129.

231. Kneepkens AFLM, Macdonald AA. Cranial Muscles of the Sulawesi Babirusa ( Babyrousa celebensis ). Anat Histol Embryol. 2010; 39: 120-137.

232. Adam Z, Awaad A, Tawfiek M, Ibrahim A. Gross anatomy of the narial and labial musculatures of one humped Camel (Camelus dromedarius). J Morphol Sci. 2016; 33: 171-178.

233. Xing Y, Chen L, Li S. Evoked electromyography to rocuronium in orbicularis oris and gastrocnemius in facial nerve injury in rabbits. J Surg Res. 2013; 185: 198-205.

234. Ziermann JM, Jr RED, Diogo R. Heads, Jaws, and Muscles: Anatomical, Functional, and Developmental Diversity in Chordate Evolution. Springer. 2019; 313.

235. Marshall CD, Hsu RH, Herring SW. Somatotopic Organization of Perioral Musculature Innervation within the Pig Facial Motor Nucleus. Brain Behav Evol. 2005; 66: 22-34.

236. Niimi Y, Matsumine H, Fukuda S, Salsbury JR, Niimi Y, Herndon DN, et al. Surgical anatomy of ovine facial and hypoglossal nerves for facial nerve reconstruction and regeneration research: An experimental study in sheep. Microsurgery. 2020; 40: 51-58.

237. Furutani R, Izawa T, Sugita S. Distribution of Facial Motoneurons Innervating the Common Facial Muscles of the Rabbit and Rat. Okajimas Folia Anatomica Japonica. 2004; 81: 101-108.

238. Komiyama M, Shibata H, Suzuki T. Somatotopic Representation of Facial Muscles within the Facial Nucleus of the Mouse. BBE. 1984; 24: 144-151.

239. Eshra EA, Badawy AM. Peculiarities of the Camel and Sheep Narial Musculature in Relation to the Clinical Value and the Mechanism of Narial Closure. 2014; 4.

240. King JE. Some aspect of the anatomy of the Ross seal, Ommatophoca Rossi. Sci Rep. 1969.

241. Irifune T. Facial Artery of the Rabbit. Okajimas Folia Anatomica Japonica. 1986; 62: 261-79.

242. Goddard JM, Rossel M, Manley NR, Capecchi MR. Mice with targeted disruption of Hoxb-1 fail to form the motor nucleus of the VIIth nerve. Dev. 1996; 122: 12.

243. Ootaki S, Yamamura K, Inoue M, Amarasena JKC, Kurose M, Yamada Y. Activity of peri-oral facial muscles and its coordination with jaw muscles during ingestive behavior in awake rabbits. Brain Res. 2004; 1001: 22-36.

244. Shimozawa A, Ishizuya-Oka A. Muscle fiber type analysis in the mouse m. digastricus, m. stylohyoideus, m. zygomaticus and m. buccinator. Anat Anz. 1987; 164: 355-361.

245. Tomo S, Tomo I, Nakajima K, Townsend GC, Hirata K. Comparative anatomy of the buccinator muscle in cat (Felis domestica). Anat Rec. 2002; 267: 78-86.

246. Sachs NA, Chang EL, Vyas N, Sorensen BN, Weiland JD. Electrical Stimulation of the Paralyzed Orbicularis Oculi in Rabbit. IEEE Trans Neural Sys Rehab Eng. 2007; 15: 67-75.

247. Baisden RH, Woodruff ML, Whittington DL, Baker DC, Benson AE. Cells of origin of the branches of the facial nerve: A retrograde HRP study in the rabbit. Am J Anat. 1987; 178:175-184.

248. Sasaki R, Watanabe Y, Yamato M, Aoki S, Okano T, Ando T. Surgical anatomy of the swine face. Lab Anim. 2010; 44: 359-363.

249. Jergovi? D, Stål P, Lidman D, Lindvall B, Hildebrand C. Changes in a rat facial muscle after facial nerve injury and repair. Muscle Nerve. 2001; 24: 1202-1212.

250. Dessem D, Druzinsky RE. Jaw-muscle activity in ferrets, Mustela putorius furo. J Morphol. 1992; 213: 275-286.

251. Rayne J, Crawford GN. Increase in fibre numbers of the rat pterygoid muscles during postnatal growth. J Anat. 1975; 119: 347-357.

252. Tokioka T, Ohta Y, Inazuka H, Miyake H. The Medial and Lateral Pterygoid Muscles of the Rabbit. Okajimas Folia Anatomica Japonica. 1982; 58: 1135-1147.

253. Abe S, Hiroki E, Iwanuma O, Sakiyama K, Shirakura Y, Hirose D, et al. Relationship between Function of Masticatory Muscle in Mouse and Properties of Muscle Fibers. Bull Tokyo Dent Coll. 2008; 49: 53-58.

254. Badawi H, El-Shaieb M, Kenawy A. The Arteria maxillaris of the Camel (Camelus dromedarius). Anatom Histol Embryol. 1977; 6: 21- 28.

255. Jerbi H, Pérez W. Descriptive anatomy of artery of one-humped camel head (Camelus dromedarius). MOJAP. 2018; 5.

256. Waller BM, Julle-Daniere E, Micheletta J. Measuring the evolution of facial ‘expression’ using multi-species FACS. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2020; 113: 1-11.

257. Ekman R. What the Face Reveals: Basic and Applied Studies of Spontaneous Expression Using the Facial Action Coding System (FACS). Oxford Univ Press. 1997; 514.

Pellon S, Hallegot M., Lapique J. and Tomberg C (2020) A Redefinition of Facial Communication in Non-Human Animals. J Behav 3(1): 1017.

Received : 22 Oct 2020
Accepted : 17 Nov 2020
Published : 21 Nov 2020
Journals
Annals of Otolaryngology and Rhinology
ISSN : 2379-948X
Launched : 2014
JSM Schizophrenia
Launched : 2016
Journal of Nausea
Launched : 2020
JSM Internal Medicine
Launched : 2016
JSM Hepatitis
Launched : 2016
JSM Oro Facial Surgeries
ISSN : 2578-3211
Launched : 2016
Journal of Human Nutrition and Food Science
ISSN : 2333-6706
Launched : 2013
JSM Regenerative Medicine and Bioengineering
ISSN : 2379-0490
Launched : 2013
JSM Spine
ISSN : 2578-3181
Launched : 2016
Archives of Palliative Care
ISSN : 2573-1165
Launched : 2016
JSM Nutritional Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3203
Launched : 2017
Annals of Neurodegenerative Disorders
ISSN : 2476-2032
Launched : 2016
Journal of Fever
ISSN : 2641-7782
Launched : 2017
JSM Bone Marrow Research
ISSN : 2578-3351
Launched : 2016
JSM Mathematics and Statistics
ISSN : 2578-3173
Launched : 2014
Journal of Autoimmunity and Research
ISSN : 2573-1173
Launched : 2014
JSM Arthritis
ISSN : 2475-9155
Launched : 2016
JSM Head and Neck Cancer-Cases and Reviews
ISSN : 2573-1610
Launched : 2016
JSM General Surgery Cases and Images
ISSN : 2573-1564
Launched : 2016
JSM Anatomy and Physiology
ISSN : 2573-1262
Launched : 2016
JSM Dental Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1548
Launched : 2016
Annals of Emergency Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1017
Launched : 2016
Annals of Mens Health and Wellness
ISSN : 2641-7707
Launched : 2017
Journal of Preventive Medicine and Health Care
ISSN : 2576-0084
Launched : 2018
Journal of Chronic Diseases and Management
ISSN : 2573-1300
Launched : 2016
Annals of Vaccines and Immunization
ISSN : 2378-9379
Launched : 2014
JSM Heart Surgery Cases and Images
ISSN : 2578-3157
Launched : 2016
Annals of Reproductive Medicine and Treatment
ISSN : 2573-1092
Launched : 2016
JSM Brain Science
ISSN : 2573-1289
Launched : 2016
JSM Biomarkers
ISSN : 2578-3815
Launched : 2014
JSM Biology
ISSN : 2475-9392
Launched : 2016
Archives of Stem Cell and Research
ISSN : 2578-3580
Launched : 2014
Annals of Clinical and Medical Microbiology
ISSN : 2578-3629
Launched : 2014
JSM Pediatric Surgery
ISSN : 2578-3149
Launched : 2017
Journal of Memory Disorder and Rehabilitation
ISSN : 2578-319X
Launched : 2016
JSM Tropical Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2578-3165
Launched : 2016
JSM Head and Face Medicine
ISSN : 2578-3793
Launched : 2016
JSM Cardiothoracic Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1297
Launched : 2016
JSM Bone and Joint Diseases
ISSN : 2578-3351
Launched : 2017
JSM Bioavailability and Bioequivalence
ISSN : 2641-7812
Launched : 2017
JSM Atherosclerosis
ISSN : 2573-1270
Launched : 2016
Journal of Genitourinary Disorders
ISSN : 2641-7790
Launched : 2017
Journal of Fractures and Sprains
ISSN : 2578-3831
Launched : 2016
Journal of Autism and Epilepsy
ISSN : 2641-7774
Launched : 2016
Annals of Marine Biology and Research
ISSN : 2573-105X
Launched : 2014
JSM Health Education & Primary Health Care
ISSN : 2578-3777
Launched : 2016
JSM Communication Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3807
Launched : 2016
Annals of Musculoskeletal Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3599
Launched : 2016
Annals of Virology and Research
ISSN : 2573-1122
Launched : 2014
JSM Renal Medicine
ISSN : 2573-1637
Launched : 2016
Journal of Muscle Health
ISSN : 2578-3823
Launched : 2016
JSM Genetics and Genomics
ISSN : 2334-1823
Launched : 2013
JSM Anxiety and Depression
ISSN : 2475-9139
Launched : 2016
Clinical Journal of Heart Diseases
ISSN : 2641-7766
Launched : 2016
Annals of Medicinal Chemistry and Research
ISSN : 2378-9336
Launched : 2014
JSM Pain and Management
ISSN : 2578-3378
Launched : 2016
JSM Women's Health
ISSN : 2578-3696
Launched : 2016
Clinical Research in HIV or AIDS
ISSN : 2374-0094
Launched : 2013
Journal of Endocrinology, Diabetes and Obesity
ISSN : 2333-6692
Launched : 2013
Journal of Substance Abuse and Alcoholism
ISSN : 2373-9363
Launched : 2013
JSM Neurosurgery and Spine
ISSN : 2373-9479
Launched : 2013
Journal of Liver and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2379-0830
Launched : 2014
Journal of Drug Design and Research
ISSN : 2379-089X
Launched : 2014
JSM Clinical Oncology and Research
ISSN : 2373-938X
Launched : 2013
JSM Bioinformatics, Genomics and Proteomics
ISSN : 2576-1102
Launched : 2014
JSM Chemistry
ISSN : 2334-1831
Launched : 2013
Journal of Trauma and Care
ISSN : 2573-1246
Launched : 2014
JSM Surgical Oncology and Research
ISSN : 2578-3688
Launched : 2016
Annals of Food Processing and Preservation
ISSN : 2573-1033
Launched : 2016
Journal of Radiology and Radiation Therapy
ISSN : 2333-7095
Launched : 2013
JSM Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation
ISSN : 2578-3572
Launched : 2016
Annals of Clinical Pathology
ISSN : 2373-9282
Launched : 2013
Annals of Cardiovascular Diseases
ISSN : 2641-7731
Launched : 2016
Annals of Clinical and Experimental Metabolism
ISSN : 2572-2492
Launched : 2016
Clinical Research in Infectious Diseases
ISSN : 2379-0636
Launched : 2013
JSM Microbiology
ISSN : 2333-6455
Launched : 2013
Journal of Urology and Research
ISSN : 2379-951X
Launched : 2014
Journal of Family Medicine and Community Health
ISSN : 2379-0547
Launched : 2013
Annals of Pregnancy and Care
ISSN : 2578-336X
Launched : 2017
JSM Cell and Developmental Biology
ISSN : 2379-061X
Launched : 2013
Annals of Aquaculture and Research
ISSN : 2379-0881
Launched : 2014
Clinical Research in Pulmonology
ISSN : 2333-6625
Launched : 2013
Journal of Immunology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2333-6714
Launched : 2013
Annals of Forensic Research and Analysis
ISSN : 2378-9476
Launched : 2014
JSM Biochemistry and Molecular Biology
ISSN : 2333-7109
Launched : 2013
Annals of Breast Cancer Research
ISSN : 2641-7685
Launched : 2016
Annals of Gerontology and Geriatric Research
ISSN : 2378-9409
Launched : 2014
Journal of Sleep Medicine and Disorders
ISSN : 2379-0822
Launched : 2014
JSM Burns and Trauma
ISSN : 2475-9406
Launched : 2016
Chemical Engineering and Process Techniques
ISSN : 2333-6633
Launched : 2013
Annals of Clinical Cytology and Pathology
ISSN : 2475-9430
Launched : 2014
JSM Allergy and Asthma
ISSN : 2573-1254
Launched : 2016
Journal of Neurological Disorders and Stroke
ISSN : 2334-2307
Launched : 2013
Annals of Sports Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2379-0571
Launched : 2014
JSM Sexual Medicine
ISSN : 2578-3718
Launched : 2016
Annals of Vascular Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2378-9344
Launched : 2014
JSM Biotechnology and Biomedical Engineering
ISSN : 2333-7117
Launched : 2013
Journal of Hematology and Transfusion
ISSN : 2333-6684
Launched : 2013
JSM Environmental Science and Ecology
ISSN : 2333-7141
Launched : 2013
Journal of Cardiology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2333-6676
Launched : 2013
JSM Nanotechnology and Nanomedicine
ISSN : 2334-1815
Launched : 2013
Journal of Ear, Nose and Throat Disorders
ISSN : 2475-9473
Launched : 2016
JSM Ophthalmology
ISSN : 2333-6447
Launched : 2013
Journal of Pharmacology and Clinical Toxicology
ISSN : 2333-7079
Launched : 2013
Annals of Psychiatry and Mental Health
ISSN : 2374-0124
Launched : 2013
Medical Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology
ISSN : 2333-6439
Launched : 2013
Annals of Pediatrics and Child Health
ISSN : 2373-9312
Launched : 2013
JSM Clinical Pharmaceutics
ISSN : 2379-9498
Launched : 2014
JSM Foot and Ankle
ISSN : 2475-9112
Launched : 2016
JSM Alzheimer's Disease and Related Dementia
ISSN : 2378-9565
Launched : 2014
Journal of Addiction Medicine and Therapy
ISSN : 2333-665X
Launched : 2013
Journal of Veterinary Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2378-931X
Launched : 2013
Annals of Public Health and Research
ISSN : 2378-9328
Launched : 2014
Annals of Orthopedics and Rheumatology
ISSN : 2373-9290
Launched : 2013
Journal of Clinical Nephrology and Research
ISSN : 2379-0652
Launched : 2014
Annals of Community Medicine and Practice
ISSN : 2475-9465
Launched : 2014
Annals of Biometrics and Biostatistics
ISSN : 2374-0116
Launched : 2013
JSM Clinical Case Reports
ISSN : 2373-9819
Launched : 2013
Journal of Cancer Biology and Research
ISSN : 2373-9436
Launched : 2013
Journal of Surgery and Transplantation Science
ISSN : 2379-0911
Launched : 2013
Journal of Dermatology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2373-9371
Launched : 2013
JSM Gastroenterology and Hepatology
ISSN : 2373-9487
Launched : 2013
Annals of Nursing and Practice
ISSN : 2379-9501
Launched : 2014
JSM Dentistry
ISSN : 2333-7133
Launched : 2013
Author Information X