Loading

Journal of Behavior

Anger: Its Impact on Discrete Emotions and Posttraumatic Stress

Original Research | Open Access | Volume 1 | Issue 1

  • 1. University of Washington Tacoma, Tacoma, USA
  • 2. Seattle Pacific University, Seattle, USA
+ Show More - Show Less
Corresponding Authors
Amanda Edwards-Stewart, 9933 West Hayes St., JBLM, Tacoma, WA 98431, USA, Tel: +1-253-968-3420; Fax: +1-253-968-4192
Abstract

Anger is often presumed to be a defensive emotion, masking the ability to feel other discrete emotions. College students (N = 176) were randomly assigned to anger priming via feigned extra credit revocation, autobiographical recall, or no priming. Students then watched the Discrete Emotion Video Clips and reported how much they felt a range of emotions on the Post Film Questionnaire. Those assigned to either of the anger priming conditions reported significantly greater amusement intensity following the associated movie clip viewing than those not anger primed (p < .05). For a subset of participants who completed the Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Checklist (n = 109), amusement negatively predicted Posttraumatic Stress Disorder symptom intensity (r = -.22; p < .05). These findings bring into question the clinical assumption that anger is used to avoid or mask feelings of disgust, fear, or sadness as no relationship was found between anger priming and these negative discrete emotions. Findings also raise important questions for future research.

Keywords

• Anger
• State anger
• Trait anger
• Discrete emotions
• Posttraumatic stress

Citation

Edwards-Stewart A, Keller CJ, Ahmad ZS, June J, Hamann R, McBrearty M (2016) Anger: Its Impact on Discrete Emotions and Posttraumatic Stress. J Behav 1(1): 1001

INTRODUCTION

Chronic habitual anger may limit an emotional range or dampen an individual’s ability to access other discrete moods, both positive and negative. Discrete emotions are generally agreed to include: joy (sometimes discussed as happiness or amusement), fear, sadness, anger [1,2], contentment, disgust, surprise, and anxiety [3]. The theory that one emotion may function to truncate one’s emotional range (i.e., suppress others) is a phenomenon identified in some cognitive and behavioral therapies, such as Emotion-Focused Therapy (EFT) [4] and Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT) [5].

Problematic anger is the focus of this study. Manifested in several ways, problematic anger includes hate, rage, destructive anger, and rejecting anger [6]. Using an EFT frame, problem anger can develop through many pathogenic pathways. Often maladaptive or inflexible primary emotions originate from childhood where the emotions were once adaptive. For example, if a child learned to feel angry to scare off potential abusers that anger was once very adaptive. In contract, using anger as an adult when not feeling emotionally safe is maladaptive as it no longer advances the relational safety needs of an adult. Therefore, maladaptive emotions are negative emotional experiences that occur repeatedly, are self-perpetuating, and are resistant to change [7]. Both primary and secondary anger may become characterological or a trait, rather than a discrete affective event or a state reaction. For instance, anger can lead to a relational style over time that is fundamentally rejecting of others. Through reinforcement, this pattern becomes a characterological way of being. This phenomenon is often exhibited in individuals with a complex trauma history; habitual anger reflects the emotional pain that has been endured historically while simultaneously serving a protective function by signaling others to keep a distance [8]. Anger, therefore, can manifest as a defensive strategy [6,9].

Within an ACT conceptualization, the emotion of anger may function to avoid a wider range of emotions. Anger may serve to perpetuate cognitive fusion (i.e., being unable to adaptively distance one’s self from thoughts) and experiential avoidance (i.e., pushing away internal or external negative experiences). Problem anger, according to ACT, is understood not so much to be a problem with the emotion of anger but the reactive behaviors typically associated with anger [10]. Accepting the emotion of anger may allow an individual to be more reflective about their experience, cultivating a posture of openness toward other emotions. Through the acceptance of the emotion of anger, individuals can develop psychological flexibility through mindfully focusing on adaptive, value-consistent behaviors. Often avoidant behaviors perpetuate the suppression of emotion requiring ongoing avoidant strategies. Consistent with an ACT perspective, the current investigation hypothesizes that problem anger functions to dampen primary emotions that are both negative and positive.

The goal of this study was to explore the relationship between problematic anger (measured as trait anger) and its impact on ability to feel other discrete emotions. State anger is also measured to induce determine if non-characterological anger alone affects ability to feel other moods. Two hypotheses were explored. The first hypothesis (H1) is that state anger, when primed, reduces reported experiences of positive and negative discrete emotions; the second hypothesis (H2) is that trait anger decreases reported experiences of positive and negative emotions.

Because defensive anger has been noted to be associated with trauma [8], the PCL-C was given to 109 of the participants. For these participants, two additional hypotheses were investigated: (H3) Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) symptoms, when primed with state anger, reduce reported experiences of positive and negative discrete emotions and (H4) PTSD symptoms and trait anger impact ability to feel both positive and negative discrete emotions. This secondary study used the same methodology as the first with the inclusion of a self-report assessment of PTSD symptoms.

Abbreviations

EFT: Emotion-focused therapy; ACT: Acceptance and commitment therapy; PTSD: Posttraumatic stress disorder; STAXI-2: State-trait anger expression inventory-2; PCL: Posttraumatic checklist-civilian version; ANOVA: One way analysis of variance; MANOVA: One-way multivariate analysis of varian

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Participants

Participants in the present study were undergraduate students recruited from a large public state university in Western Washington. Students were invited from several subject areas (including psychology and mathematics) and given extra credit in exchange for their participation. The majority of students in invited classes participated. Participants ranged in age from 18 to 49 with a mean age of 23. The majority of the sample was Caucasian (49%) followed by 40% African American, 5% Asian, and 5% biracial. The sample was 52% women.

Procedure

This study was approved by the University of Washington Institutional Review Board. The study consisted of 176 participants. Participants reported to a sign up table and were given a consent form and time to individually ask questions about the study. Participants were randomly assigned to one of three conditions. The first condition aimed to induce anger by telling students they did a poor job interpreting a poem and would therefore lose extra credit points (a novel anger induction that was found in the below manipulation check to induce state anger). The second condition was also an anger induction condition which had participants recall an autobiographical time of anger (a common form of mood induction) [11] and the third condition was the control. Two forms of anger priming were used to determine whether deception in anger induction might be more effective than recall of anger. While the deception method might have also elicited other emotions (depression, anxiety, etc.), state anger was found in the below manipulation check to be present. The procedure varied based on condition, however all conditions included: initial priming or no priming state (priming conditions are described below), completing a demographic questionnaire, viewing seven movie clips designed to elicit different mood states and completing the Post Film Questionnaire [3], completing the State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory-2 (STAXI-2), and being debriefed on the actual nature of the study.

Participants in condition one were asked to write a response essay to a poem (Fire and Ice, by Robert Frost) and were informed that the quality of their response (rated as ‘good,’ ‘neutral,’ or ‘poor’) would determine the amount of extra credit they received. In this condition, state anger was invoked by each participant receiving a “poor” grade for their essay, regardless of the quality of their essay. Specifically, participants received a sealed envelope informing them of both their ‘poor’ work and decreased amount of extra credit (5 out of the possible 10). In the second condition, participants were asked to write a brief essay recalling an auto-biographical experience of anger. Participants were asked to try and feel those angry feelings in the present. In the third, control condition, participants received the same poem as in condition one and were asked to write an essay in response to the poem. However, in this condition, they were not given feedback on their written response nor were they told that their essays would be graded.

Of the total of 176 participants, 109 participants completed the Posttraumatic Checklist-Civilian Version (PCL-C) following their completion of the STAXI-2 and before they were debriefed. Sixty-seven participants did not complete the PCL-C.

Instruments

Demographic questionnaire: Participants completed a brief demographic questionnaire asking for the participant’s age, gender, race, current class standing in college, relationship status, and religious affiliation.

State-trait anger. The STAXI-2 consists of 57 items, with six scales, five subscales, and an Anger Expression Index. It was created to provide a measure of expression and control of anger. This measure was developed to assess anger as it distinguishes between expressions of normal versus abnormal anger and serves as a way to measure various aspects of anger as related to various medical conditions shown to relate to anger. Alpha co-efficient measures of internal consistency identified consistently good reliability across all scales and sub-scales (.84 or higher with a median of .88) and validity was also found to be appropriately established [12].

Discrete emotion video clips: Gross and Levenson (1995) created a series of eight film clips to elicit emotional responses in a laboratory setting. These emotional states are: amusement, anger, contentment, disgust, fear, neutral, sadness, and surprise. Each clip was determined to have varying statistical ability to induce the target emotion (entitled the clips “hit rate”) which was determined using the Post Film Questionnaire. This questionnaire consists of a list of various emotion states. The participant is asked to rate how much they felt each emotion while watching the film according to a 9-point Likert scale ranging from 0 (not at all/none) to 8 (extremely/a great deal). For the present study, all 8 clips were shown, but only four were used for analysis due to their relevance to the current topic and their high hit rate, as determined in the original study. Specifically, for amusement, the When Harry Met Sally “discussion of orgasm in café” had a hit rate of 93.1; for disgust, the Pink Flamingos “person eats dog feces” had a hit rate of 84.9; for sadness, The Champ “boy cries at father’s death” had a hit rate of 94.2; and for fear, The Shining “boy playing in hallway” had a hit rate of 71.2.

Posttraumatic Checklist-Civilian Version (PCL-C): The PCL-C [13] is a 17-item self-report measure of posttraumatic stress symptoms reflecting DSM-IV criteria for PTSD. The questions in the civilian version of this measure ask about generic stressful experiences and are meant to be used with any population. Participants rated how much they had been bothered by each symptom in the past month on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (Not at all) to 5 (Extremely). Scores range from a 17 to 85.

RESULTS

Preliminary Analyses/Manipulation Check

A manipulation check was conducted to determine if the priming conditions induced state anger as proposed. A one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted. The independent variable was condition and therefore had three levels: condition 1 (extra credit anger), condition 2 (autobiographical anger), and condition 3 (control condition). The dependent variable was state anger as measured by the STAXI-2. The ANOVA was significant, F(2, 167) = 4.76, p = .01, η² = .05. The strength of the relationship between condition and state anger was relatively small overall, only accounting for 5% of the variance. Follow-up tests were conducted to evaluate pairwise differences among the means (see Table 1). There was a significant difference between condition 1 and condition 3, as well as condition 2 and condition 3. This indicates that both condition 1 and 2 created elevated state anger compared to the control.

Main Analysis

H1. A one-way ANOVA was conducted to evaluate the mean difference between condition and the positive discrete emotion, amusement. In all hypotheses, the positive discrete emotion (amusement) was explored separately from the three negative emotions (disgust, fear, and sadness). This was done due to the varying valence between positive and negative emotions. The independent variable, conditions, had three levels. The dependent variable was level of amusement reported. The ANOVA was significant, F(2, 168) = 3.27, p < .05, η² = .04. The strength of relationship between condition and reported level of amusement (as assessed by η²) was small with the condition accounting for four percent of the variance in level of reported amusement.

Follow-up tests were conducted to evaluate pairwise differences among the means. There was a significant difference in means between both conditions 1 and 2 compared to condition 3. Both conditions showed increased reported levels of amusement compared to the control. There was no significant difference between conditions 1 and 2. The pairwise differences in means and standard deviations for the three conditions by amusement are reported in Figure 1.

Figure 1: Changes in reported amusement levels by conditio

Figure 1: Changes in reported amusement levels by conditio

A one-way multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was conducted to determine the effect of conditions on the three dependent variables of disgust, fear, and sadness. No significant differences were found among the conditions on the dependent measures, Wilks’s Λ = .95, F(2,164) = 1.43, p = .20. The multivariate η² based on Wilks’s Λ was small (.03).

H2. A series of linear regression analyses were conducted to evaluate the prediction that trait anger would have an impact on ability to feel the discrete emotions currently under investigation (amusement, disgust, fear, and sadness). None of the regressions were significant (see Table 2).

H3. This hypothesis seeks to explore if PTSD symptoms impact ability to experience various discrete emotions. Given this and the method used in the current study, an initial correlation was conducted between PTSD symptoms and state anger to identify if this might confound the measurement of PTSD symptoms as they relate to primed anger. The correlation coefficient computed between PTSD symptoms and state anger was .14, displaying a minimal positive, linear relationship between PTSD symptoms and state anger. This finding was not significant, p = .15.

PCL scores range from 17 to 85 with a mean score of 35 and standard deviation of 15. Five linear regression analyses were conducted to predict PCL scores impact on level of reported discrete emotions. The analysis including amusement was significant while the others comprised of anger, disgust, fear, and sadness were not significant (see Table 3 for non-significant regression equations). Anger was included in this analysis to determine if experience of anger was different based on PCL-C scores. Experience of anger was collected by the Discrete Emotion Video Clips by Gross and Levenson (1995) as was the case for all participants.

The scatterplot for the PCL and amusement indicates that the two variables are linearly related such that as PCL scores increase amusement decreases. The regression equation was significant, R2 = .05, F(1,106) = 5.14, p < .05. The regression equation for predicting the overall injury index is

Predicted Overall PCL Score = -.05 Overall Amusement = 7.9

The 95% confidence interval for the slope, -.10 to -.01, does not contain the value of zero, therefore the PCL is significantly related to amusement. The correlation between PCL and amusement was -.22. Approximately five percent of the variance of amusement was accounted for by its linear relationship with PCL scores.

H4. A correlation coefficient was computed between PTSD symptoms and trait anger to confirm past research findings that the two variables were related. The results were statistically significant (r = .37, p < .01.

A series of linear regression analyses were conducted to evaluate the prediction that PTSD symptoms and trait anger would have an impact on ability to feel the discrete emotions currently under investigation (amusement and disgust, fear, and sadness). None of the regressions were significant (see Table 4).

Tables 1: 95% Confidence intervals of pairwise differences in mean changes in state anger

Conditions   M  SD Condition 1 Condition 2
Condition 1 49.37 .94    
Condition 2 50.68 .96    
Condition 3 46.49 .99 .17 to 5.59 1.46 to 6.93
Abbreviations: M: Mean; SD: Standard Deviation

Table 2: Regression equations of trait anger by discrete emotion

Discrete emotion  R  F  df p value
Amusement 0.03 0.15 -1,167 0.69
Disgust 0.01 0.01 -1,167 0.94
Fear 0.02 0.05 -1,165 0.82
Sadness 0.04 0.3 -1,164 0.58

Table 3: Non-significant regression equations for PCL by negative discrete emotion

Discrete emotion  R  F   df p value
Amusement .09 .92 (1,105) .34
Disgust .01 .02 (1,105) .89
Fear .06 .38 (1,105) .54
Sadness .13 1.74 (1,105) .19

Table 4: Non-significant regression equations of PTSD symptoms and trait anger by discrete emotion

Discrete emotion  R  F   df p value
Amusement .15 1.19 (2,103) .31
Disgust .11 .60 (2,102) .55
Fear .13 .90 (2,102) .41
Sadness .15 1.19 (2,102) .31
DISCUSSION

This study explored how anger, state and trait, influenced reported experiences of various discrete emotions. We explored both positive and negative emotions. We predicted that primed anger would decrease reported experiences of negative emotions such as fear or sadness. If such a relationship is present, it arguably could differ for non-clinical groups versus clinical groups. However, for the initial exploration of this assumption, a non-clinical group was chosen to determine if this was a typical experience for any individual regardless of diagnosis or syndrome. Further research would need to explore this assumption by including different clinical populations in the sample. Positive emotions are not typically identified in any assumptions on anger and discrete emotions but the present study wished to explore this dimension. If anger does indeed dampen various negative emotions, perhaps it also affects the experience of positive ones. This exploration was indeed the most interesting in the present study, as amusement was the only significant variable regardless of the independent variable.

In regards to state anger, it appears that when a non-clinical group is primed, they experience higher levels of amusement than if they were not. While the explained variance between the variables is small, meaning there are other variables that impact amusement levels beyond state anger, it appears that higher state anger shortened the reported range of experience of amusement. When considering Figure A, it appears that individuals in conditions 1 and 2 all reported amusement in response to the video stimuli where condition 3, the control, had a full range of reported amusement. While the mean levels of amusement were significantly different between the primed and control, a few individuals in the control reported no amusement when viewing the stimulus which was not the case in the primed conditions. A possible explanation for this finding could be that emotions, physiologically speaking, are indistinct. It is only cognitions and interpretation of emotional stimuli that differentiate the different mood states. Therefore, anger is similar to amusement in that one likely experiencing increased heart rate, breathing, and muscle tension while having these emotions and to be primed to feel anger can easily be confused with experiencing amusement when the stimulus changes.

In terms of problematic or trait anger and its impact on positive and negative discrete emotions, there were no significant results. This indicates that characterological anger that was represented in the current sample (trait anger had a range of T scores from 74-32 with a mean of 48), does not appear to have a dampening relationship with reported experiences of the explored positive and negative emotions. Given the theoretic assumptions around problematic anger and its impact on other negative mood states, it appears that for a non-clinical population, simply priming those with and without problematic/trait anger does not globally diminish ability to feel these emotions. These results could be artifacts of an experimental design, where discrete emotions were elicited outside of a personally meaningful context that did not naturally trigger defensive anger.

In regards to the second aim of this study and the relationship between PTSD symptoms and discrete emotions, it appears that as PCL scores increase, reported amusement decreases. This indicates that something about PTSD symptoms, in a non-clinical population, has the opposite impact on amusement than primed state anger alone. It appears that the indistinct view of emotions postulated above might not apply to a group with PTSD symptoms. This may be due to the diagnostic criteria for PTSD that identifies a persistent inability to experience positive emotions. The explained variance in this study was small, however, indicating that other variables likely explain reported experiences of amusement other than PTSD symptoms.

It was interesting that reported experiences of disgust, fear, or sadness were not influenced by increases in PTSD symptoms. This simply indicates that as PTSD symptoms increase, this nonclinical group did not report higher or lower experiences of negative discrete emotions than those with no PTSD symptoms. While this finding is interesting because it is the first of its kind, it much also be treated with caution. This data is inherently limited as it was explored among a non-clinical population, thereby the actual amount of those with PTSD symptoms were small (a total of 21 participants out of 109 had a PCL-C score of 50 or above), and the methodology of this study was also designed with a priming condition of state anger. The current exploration should therefore be explored with a clinical group.

Finally, the current study supports past research that trait anger has a positive relationship with PTSD symptoms [14]. However, it does not appear that defensive or trait anger and PTSD symptoms influence reported experiences of any of the positive or negative discrete emotions currently under investigation. This indicates that trait anger and PTSD symptoms do not appear to have an impact on experiences of amusement, anger, disgust, fear, or sadness.

CONCLUSION

The limitations of the current study are the relatively small effect sizes. To the authors’ knowledge, this study is the first of its kind, and thereby important to the field. There are, however, other factors that likely explain an individual’s ability to feel amusement in any given moment. Therefore, the present study can only serve to illuminate a novel concept that has not been previously investigated. This study should not be considered as fully elucidating the relationship between state and trait anger, nor the relationship between defensive anger and ability to feel discrete emotions. Further limitations involve the method used in the current study. Anger could not be measured before priming since deception was used in one of the priming conditions. Doing so would have made the participants aware of the context of the study, and at least for condition 1, this could not occur. Since it appears that condition 1 and 2 were comparable in priming anger, this deception might not be necessary. In future research defensive anger should be explored with different clinical samples to identify if primed defensive anger can affect ability to feel other vulnerable negative mood states such as shame. For a non-clinical group, primed defensive anger does not appear to have this kind of global impact on negative emotions.

In summary, this study was the first to investigate this theoretical concept experimentally and therefore has value but follow-up research is needed to explore the relationship between problematic or trait anger in a clinical population, using priming conditions that elicit personally meaningful defensive anger, and its impact on ability to feel other discrete emotions.

DISCLOSURE

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

REFERENCES

1. Frijda NH. Emotion, cognitive structure, and action tendency. Cognition Emotion. 1987; 1: 115-143.

2. Lazarus RS. Relational Meaning and Discrete Emotions. In Scherer KR, Schorr A, Johnstone T, editors. Appraisal processes in emotion: theory, methods, research. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 2001: 37-67.

3. Gross JJ, Levenson RW. Emotion elicitation using films. Cognition Emotion. 1995; 9: 87-108.

4. Greenberg LS. Emotion-focused Therapy. Clin Psychol Psychot. 2004; 11: 3-16.

5. Hayes SC, Strosahl KD, Wilson KG. Acceptance and Commitment Therapy, Second Edition: The Process and Practice of Mindful Change. New York: The Guilford Press; 2012.

6. Pascual-Leone A, Gillis P, Singh T, Andreescu CA. Problem anger in psychotherapy: An emotion-focused perspective on hate, rage, and rejecting anger. J Contemp Psychot. 2013; 43: 83-92.

7. Greenberg LS, Paivio SC. Working with emotions in psychotherapy. New York: Guilford Press; 1997.

8. Paivio SC, Pascual-Leone A. Emotion focused therapy for complex trauma: An integrative approach. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2010.

9. Korman LM. Treating anger and addictions concurrently. In Skinner WJ, editor. Treating concurrent disorders: A guide for counselors. Toronto: Centre for Addict Ment Hlt; 2005: 215-234.

10. Eifert GH, McKay M, Forsyth JP. Act on life not on anger: The new acceptance & commitment therapy guide to problem anger. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger Publications; 2006.

11. Jallais C, Gilet A. Inducing changes in arousal and valence: Comparison of two mood induction procedures. Behav Res Meth. 2010; 42: 318- 325.

12. Spielberger CD. State-trait anger expression inventory-2: Professional Manual. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc; 1999.

13. Weathers FW, Huska JA, Keane TM. PCL-C for DSM-IV. Boston: National Center for PTSD- Behavioral Science Division; 1991.

14. Meffert SM, Metzler TJ, Henn-Haase C, McCaslin S, Inslicht S, Chemtob C, Neylan T, Marmar CR. A prospective study of trait anger and PTSD symptoms in police. J Trauma Stress. 2008; 21: 410-416.

About the Corresponding Author

Dr. Amanda Edwards-Stewart

Summary of background:

Clinical Psychologist currently working for a Department of Defense organization. Also, works as adjunct faculty at Northwest University and maintains a private practice in Seattle, WA.

Current research focus:

• Media contagion
• Virtual reality exposure therapy
• The use of technology to improve service members behavioral health,and posttraumatic stress outcomes in outpatient military behavioralhealth clinics

Websites

ResearchGate - https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Amanda_ Edwards-Stewart

LinkedIn - https://www.linkedin.com/in/amanda-stewart-729b1355

Permanent e-mail address: amandaedwardsstewart@gmail.com

Received : 26 May 2016
Accepted : 25 Jul 2016
Published : 16 Sep 2016
Journals
Annals of Otolaryngology and Rhinology
ISSN : 2379-948X
Launched : 2014
JSM Schizophrenia
Launched : 2016
Journal of Nausea
Launched : 2020
JSM Internal Medicine
Launched : 2016
JSM Hepatitis
Launched : 2016
JSM Oro Facial Surgeries
ISSN : 2578-3211
Launched : 2016
Journal of Human Nutrition and Food Science
ISSN : 2333-6706
Launched : 2013
JSM Regenerative Medicine and Bioengineering
ISSN : 2379-0490
Launched : 2013
JSM Spine
ISSN : 2578-3181
Launched : 2016
Archives of Palliative Care
ISSN : 2573-1165
Launched : 2016
JSM Nutritional Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3203
Launched : 2017
Annals of Neurodegenerative Disorders
ISSN : 2476-2032
Launched : 2016
Journal of Fever
ISSN : 2641-7782
Launched : 2017
JSM Bone Marrow Research
ISSN : 2578-3351
Launched : 2016
JSM Mathematics and Statistics
ISSN : 2578-3173
Launched : 2014
Journal of Autoimmunity and Research
ISSN : 2573-1173
Launched : 2014
JSM Arthritis
ISSN : 2475-9155
Launched : 2016
JSM Head and Neck Cancer-Cases and Reviews
ISSN : 2573-1610
Launched : 2016
JSM General Surgery Cases and Images
ISSN : 2573-1564
Launched : 2016
JSM Anatomy and Physiology
ISSN : 2573-1262
Launched : 2016
JSM Dental Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1548
Launched : 2016
Annals of Emergency Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1017
Launched : 2016
Annals of Mens Health and Wellness
ISSN : 2641-7707
Launched : 2017
Journal of Preventive Medicine and Health Care
ISSN : 2576-0084
Launched : 2018
Journal of Chronic Diseases and Management
ISSN : 2573-1300
Launched : 2016
Annals of Vaccines and Immunization
ISSN : 2378-9379
Launched : 2014
JSM Heart Surgery Cases and Images
ISSN : 2578-3157
Launched : 2016
Annals of Reproductive Medicine and Treatment
ISSN : 2573-1092
Launched : 2016
JSM Brain Science
ISSN : 2573-1289
Launched : 2016
JSM Biomarkers
ISSN : 2578-3815
Launched : 2014
JSM Biology
ISSN : 2475-9392
Launched : 2016
Archives of Stem Cell and Research
ISSN : 2578-3580
Launched : 2014
Annals of Clinical and Medical Microbiology
ISSN : 2578-3629
Launched : 2014
JSM Pediatric Surgery
ISSN : 2578-3149
Launched : 2017
Journal of Memory Disorder and Rehabilitation
ISSN : 2578-319X
Launched : 2016
JSM Tropical Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2578-3165
Launched : 2016
JSM Head and Face Medicine
ISSN : 2578-3793
Launched : 2016
JSM Cardiothoracic Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1297
Launched : 2016
JSM Bone and Joint Diseases
ISSN : 2578-3351
Launched : 2017
JSM Bioavailability and Bioequivalence
ISSN : 2641-7812
Launched : 2017
JSM Atherosclerosis
ISSN : 2573-1270
Launched : 2016
Journal of Genitourinary Disorders
ISSN : 2641-7790
Launched : 2017
Journal of Fractures and Sprains
ISSN : 2578-3831
Launched : 2016
Journal of Autism and Epilepsy
ISSN : 2641-7774
Launched : 2016
Annals of Marine Biology and Research
ISSN : 2573-105X
Launched : 2014
JSM Health Education & Primary Health Care
ISSN : 2578-3777
Launched : 2016
JSM Communication Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3807
Launched : 2016
Annals of Musculoskeletal Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3599
Launched : 2016
Annals of Virology and Research
ISSN : 2573-1122
Launched : 2014
JSM Renal Medicine
ISSN : 2573-1637
Launched : 2016
Journal of Muscle Health
ISSN : 2578-3823
Launched : 2016
JSM Genetics and Genomics
ISSN : 2334-1823
Launched : 2013
JSM Anxiety and Depression
ISSN : 2475-9139
Launched : 2016
Clinical Journal of Heart Diseases
ISSN : 2641-7766
Launched : 2016
Annals of Medicinal Chemistry and Research
ISSN : 2378-9336
Launched : 2014
JSM Pain and Management
ISSN : 2578-3378
Launched : 2016
JSM Women's Health
ISSN : 2578-3696
Launched : 2016
Clinical Research in HIV or AIDS
ISSN : 2374-0094
Launched : 2013
Journal of Endocrinology, Diabetes and Obesity
ISSN : 2333-6692
Launched : 2013
Journal of Substance Abuse and Alcoholism
ISSN : 2373-9363
Launched : 2013
JSM Neurosurgery and Spine
ISSN : 2373-9479
Launched : 2013
Journal of Liver and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2379-0830
Launched : 2014
Journal of Drug Design and Research
ISSN : 2379-089X
Launched : 2014
JSM Clinical Oncology and Research
ISSN : 2373-938X
Launched : 2013
JSM Bioinformatics, Genomics and Proteomics
ISSN : 2576-1102
Launched : 2014
JSM Chemistry
ISSN : 2334-1831
Launched : 2013
Journal of Trauma and Care
ISSN : 2573-1246
Launched : 2014
JSM Surgical Oncology and Research
ISSN : 2578-3688
Launched : 2016
Annals of Food Processing and Preservation
ISSN : 2573-1033
Launched : 2016
Journal of Radiology and Radiation Therapy
ISSN : 2333-7095
Launched : 2013
JSM Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation
ISSN : 2578-3572
Launched : 2016
Annals of Clinical Pathology
ISSN : 2373-9282
Launched : 2013
Annals of Cardiovascular Diseases
ISSN : 2641-7731
Launched : 2016
Annals of Clinical and Experimental Metabolism
ISSN : 2572-2492
Launched : 2016
Clinical Research in Infectious Diseases
ISSN : 2379-0636
Launched : 2013
JSM Microbiology
ISSN : 2333-6455
Launched : 2013
Journal of Urology and Research
ISSN : 2379-951X
Launched : 2014
Journal of Family Medicine and Community Health
ISSN : 2379-0547
Launched : 2013
Annals of Pregnancy and Care
ISSN : 2578-336X
Launched : 2017
JSM Cell and Developmental Biology
ISSN : 2379-061X
Launched : 2013
Annals of Aquaculture and Research
ISSN : 2379-0881
Launched : 2014
Clinical Research in Pulmonology
ISSN : 2333-6625
Launched : 2013
Journal of Immunology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2333-6714
Launched : 2013
Annals of Forensic Research and Analysis
ISSN : 2378-9476
Launched : 2014
JSM Biochemistry and Molecular Biology
ISSN : 2333-7109
Launched : 2013
Annals of Breast Cancer Research
ISSN : 2641-7685
Launched : 2016
Annals of Gerontology and Geriatric Research
ISSN : 2378-9409
Launched : 2014
Journal of Sleep Medicine and Disorders
ISSN : 2379-0822
Launched : 2014
JSM Burns and Trauma
ISSN : 2475-9406
Launched : 2016
Chemical Engineering and Process Techniques
ISSN : 2333-6633
Launched : 2013
Annals of Clinical Cytology and Pathology
ISSN : 2475-9430
Launched : 2014
JSM Allergy and Asthma
ISSN : 2573-1254
Launched : 2016
Journal of Neurological Disorders and Stroke
ISSN : 2334-2307
Launched : 2013
Annals of Sports Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2379-0571
Launched : 2014
JSM Sexual Medicine
ISSN : 2578-3718
Launched : 2016
Annals of Vascular Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2378-9344
Launched : 2014
JSM Biotechnology and Biomedical Engineering
ISSN : 2333-7117
Launched : 2013
Journal of Hematology and Transfusion
ISSN : 2333-6684
Launched : 2013
JSM Environmental Science and Ecology
ISSN : 2333-7141
Launched : 2013
Journal of Cardiology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2333-6676
Launched : 2013
JSM Nanotechnology and Nanomedicine
ISSN : 2334-1815
Launched : 2013
Journal of Ear, Nose and Throat Disorders
ISSN : 2475-9473
Launched : 2016
JSM Ophthalmology
ISSN : 2333-6447
Launched : 2013
Journal of Pharmacology and Clinical Toxicology
ISSN : 2333-7079
Launched : 2013
Annals of Psychiatry and Mental Health
ISSN : 2374-0124
Launched : 2013
Medical Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology
ISSN : 2333-6439
Launched : 2013
Annals of Pediatrics and Child Health
ISSN : 2373-9312
Launched : 2013
JSM Clinical Pharmaceutics
ISSN : 2379-9498
Launched : 2014
JSM Foot and Ankle
ISSN : 2475-9112
Launched : 2016
JSM Alzheimer's Disease and Related Dementia
ISSN : 2378-9565
Launched : 2014
Journal of Addiction Medicine and Therapy
ISSN : 2333-665X
Launched : 2013
Journal of Veterinary Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2378-931X
Launched : 2013
Annals of Public Health and Research
ISSN : 2378-9328
Launched : 2014
Annals of Orthopedics and Rheumatology
ISSN : 2373-9290
Launched : 2013
Journal of Clinical Nephrology and Research
ISSN : 2379-0652
Launched : 2014
Annals of Community Medicine and Practice
ISSN : 2475-9465
Launched : 2014
Annals of Biometrics and Biostatistics
ISSN : 2374-0116
Launched : 2013
JSM Clinical Case Reports
ISSN : 2373-9819
Launched : 2013
Journal of Cancer Biology and Research
ISSN : 2373-9436
Launched : 2013
Journal of Surgery and Transplantation Science
ISSN : 2379-0911
Launched : 2013
Journal of Dermatology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2373-9371
Launched : 2013
JSM Gastroenterology and Hepatology
ISSN : 2373-9487
Launched : 2013
Annals of Nursing and Practice
ISSN : 2379-9501
Launched : 2014
JSM Dentistry
ISSN : 2333-7133
Launched : 2013
Author Information X