Loading

Journal of Behavior

Effects of children’s temperament on mothers’ and caregivers’ supportive reactions related to socialization of emotion regulation

Original Research | Open Access | Volume 2 | Issue 1

  • 1. Department of Social Welfare, National Chung Cheng University, Chia-Yi, Taiwan
  • 2. Department of Child Care and Education, Hungkuang University, Taichung, Taiwan
+ Show More - Show Less
Corresponding Authors
Ishien Li, Department of Child Care and Education, Hungkuang University, Shalu District, Taichung 43302, Taiwan, Tel: +886-04-2631-8652
Abstract

Emotion regulation in children is associated with various aspects of developmental outcomes. In recent decades, researchers have paid considerable attention to its socialization to identify the manner in which children’s emotion regulation may be facilitated by interaction with adults. Supportive reaction to children’s negative emotions has been found to play a crucial role in enabling children’s emotion regulation. Knowledge of the precursors of adults’ supportive reactions can help control or direct their supportive reactions in a more efficient and productive manner. We conducted this study to examine the effects of young children’s temperament on primary caregivers’ supportive reactions to the children’s negative emotions. In the first year of study (children’s mean age = 11.8 months, SD = 3.58, n=191), both the mothers and the children’s day-time caregivers completed a shortened version of the Infant Behavior Questionnaire to assess child temperament on three broad dispositional characteristics (i.e., effortful control, negative affectivity, and surgency/extraversion). One year later, both primary caregivers completed questionnaires about emotion- and problem-focused supportive reactions that they provided to their children’s negative emotions. Our results indicated that child temperament predicted certain supportive reactions of both primary caregivers. Effortful control predisposition in the children predicted both mothers’ and day-time caregivers’ emotion- and problem-focused supportive reactions. Child negative affectivity predicted lower problem-focused support of day-time caregivers, but not of mothers. Child surgency did not predict either mothers’ or caregivers’ supportive reactions. Taken together, results of this study showed that infants’ temperament could predict caregivers’ behaviors in socialization of emotion regulation.

Citation

Chang CH, Li I (2017) Effects of children’s temperament on mothers’ and caregivers’ supportive reactions related to socialization of emotion regulation. J Behav 2(1): 1007

Keywords
  • Child temperament
  • Mothers
  • Supportive reactions
  • Caregivers
INTRODUCTION

In recent decades, in order to identify the manner in which emotion regulation may be facilitated, researchers have paid considerable attention to the socialization between caregivers or parents with young children [1-4] on the ground that regulating one’s emotions and behavior is critical for success in school, work, and life [5-7]. However, the spectrum of the variants relevant to emotion regulation in young children has been under researched.

Emotion regulation of young children includes extrinsic processes, which can involve adults’ help with emotion reactivity and control [8]. In early childhood, socialization of emotion regulation from parents and other primary caregivers is one of the most critical external factors that influences young children [9]. In fact, reaction of primary caregivers to young children’s emotions plays roles in one of the major mechanisms of emotion regulation socialization. Young children’s emotions, either intense or frequent, often function to elicit reaction from their adult social partners [10-12]. Past research has already described how children’s shift from external to internal sources of control over time [13,14] in that the external support provided to the children plays a key role in how they learn to internally regulate their emotions. To sum up, supportive reactions of primary caregivers play a crucial role in providing scaffolding that enables young children’s regulation of emotions [15].

Children’s attempts to constructively regulate their negative emotions could be facilitated by adults’ supportive reactions [10,11,16]. There are two main approaches that adults use while providing support for emotion-evoked children—emotion-focused and problem-focused supportive reactions. While the first approach is rooted in emotions, the second is rooted in cognition. Emotion-focused supportive reaction refers to comforting/soothing behaviors that intend to help the child feel better [10]. Common types of comforting/soothing include hugging, rocking, patting, holding, singing, and talking soothingly [17]. If the comforting/soothing behavior is a response when children see an adult comforting their negative emotions, they may learn to use these same strategies to comfort themselves in the future. Problem-focused supportive reactions, on the other hand, include occasions when primary caregivers attempt to use cognitive regulation to reframe or reinterpret the salient features of an event that initially elicited negative emotion, in a more neutral or positive manner to help the child to cope with the problem that causes the distress [18], e.g. the adults may use cognitive regulation strategies by suggesting how the child might re-interpret key parameters of the situation (e.g., “Those dogs are barking at the cats next door to their house, not at you.”). In addition, once responses of the primary caregivers are taken into account, young children use social referencing to gain meaning about the emotional significance of events, which will allow them to begin to reinterpret negative emotional situations on their own [19,20].

A parent or caregiver may rely primarily on one of the supportive reactions, but the literature implies that optimal responses to children’s emotions involve both emotion-focused support (e.g., a warm acceptance of a child’s emotions) and problem-focused support (e.g., guidance in how to manage emotions). An emotion-focused supportive reaction may manage the emotion-evoking situation for the child and thus teaches the child that negative emotions are manageable but does not cultivate an interpersonal structure for the child’s attempts to self-regulate. Alternatively, an adult who provides primarily problem-focused support may guide the child to self-regulate but shows no warmth or tolerance of emotions which may make the child to suppress emotions or self-regulation strategy generation.

In the study of Cole and colleagues (2009), maternal emotion-focused supportive reactions was shown to increase the recognition of emotional strategies but may not increase the generation of these strategies during frustrating episodes [21]. Conversely, maternal problem-focused supportive reactions (i.e., attempts to scaffold self-regulation) were related to increased generation of strategies to cope with frustration, but not to recognition. These mixed results suggest that the relationship between socialization and children’s emotion regulation may be complex in nature, and that results are likely to have much to do with the time course of the study and the way that the behavior of socialization is conceptualized. Moreover, without a prospective design, a cross-sectional study could not reveal maternal contributions to children’s emotion regulation.

In addition, although the socialization of emotion regulation is meant to regulate a child’s conduct, it is not independent of the child’s characteristics. The relationship and interaction between child and socialization practices is bi-directional and complex [22]. A child’s temperament can be associated with the manner in which s/he regulates his/her emotions and reacts to specific caregiving behaviors. This in turn can affect the caregiving behavior of the adults [23]. Among the categories of temperamental behavioral styles that have been established with respect to childhood temperament, Rothbart and Derryberry’s temperamental dimensions have been instrumental to the field and has guided much temperament research [24]. This study adopts Rothbart’s model which conceptualizes temperament as structured into three broad clusters—effortful control, negative affectivity, and surgency/extraversion. The ways in which children’s temperament may be related to the supportive reactions of their caregivers will be discussed in the following.

Effortful control

Effortful control (EC) is defined as a dimension of temperament related to the self-regulation of emotional reactivity and behavior, reflecting individual differences in the ability to voluntarily control attention, detect errors, and activate a subdominant response in place of a more automatic/dominant response [3,24]. Previous studies have indicated the importance of EC for many developmental outcomes, including more effective emotion regulation [25]. Children with high levels of effortful control have lower parental rejection due to energy conservation and feelings of competence [26,27]. Similarly, low EC was found to predict decreasing growth trajectories of parental monitoring across childhood and adolescence [28]. Thus, low EC of young children is hypothesized to predict low supportive reactions provided by their primary caregivers.

Negative affectivity

Negative affectivity (NA) includes anger/frustration, discomfort, fear, and sadness [29]. With regard to NA, one could argue that high NA decreases supportive reaction, because negative emotions are likely to be difficult for children to control and may interfere with caregivers’ socialization efforts [30]. Repeated negatively affective behavior can lead to a frustration and feelings of insufficiency of childcare that can result in more non-supportive reactions. Thus, children with high levels of negative affectivity may make the modulation of emotional arousal difficult and may interfere with caregivers’ socialization efforts. This proposition has found support in previous research; both concurrent and longitudinal research support the association between higher intensity of child negative emotions and increased negative parenting [26,31-33]. Therefore, high NA of young children is hypothesized to predict low supportive reactions provided by their primary caregivers.

Surgency/extraversion

Surgency/extraversion is a construct that includes impulsivity, intensity pleasure, and activity level, positive anticipation, and low shyness. Little is known about the direct relationship between young children’s surgency/extraversion temperament and adult reactions to their negative emotions. However, there is some theoretical and empirical support for examining surgency/ extraversion as a risk factor for decreasing caregivers’ supportive reactions. Children with high levels of surgency/extraversion could be characterized as highly active and constantly exploring their environment with disregard for rules and little inclination to comply with requests, which may drive a caregiver to employ coercive methods of discipline, leading to frustration and decrease in supportive reactions [34]. High surgency/extraversion of young children is thus hypothesized to predict low supportive reactions provided by their primary caregivers.

It is important to examine the associations between the child’s temperament and the process of socialization of emotion regulation. Previous research has primarily focused on the effects of the adults’ supportive reactions on emotion socialization of children, but less attention has paid to the precursors of the supportive reactions. Given the fact to the significant role of the supportive reactions, more in depth research is required to study the association of children’s temperament and the socialization process in primary caregivers. This study aims to examine whether and how temperamental styles of young children affect supportive reactions of primary caregivers in dealing with their display of normally occurring negative emotions. This study investigates whether child temperament could predict two commonly used categories of supportive emotion regulation reactions - emotion-focused and problem-focused supportive reactions.

Abbreviations

EC: Effortful control; NA: Negative affectivity

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Participants

We recruited 191 mothers with a child under 18 months of age, joining in a community day-time caregiver support system sponsored by Child Welfare Bureau in Taiwan. In addition, the day-time caregivers of the children of participating mothers were also recruited. The community day-time caregiver support system was established to provide financial subsidies of child care services for working mothers with a family annual income of NT$1,130,000 (i.e., US$36,000) and below (Note: the average family annual income in Taiwan at the year of study was NT$956,849). About 48% of the mothers graduated from high school, 39% graduated from college, and 13% attended graduate school. Mother-caregiver pairs were recruited through collaboration with a local support system that served approximately 400 infants and toddlers annually. Of those eligible, 48 percent of mothers and day-time caregivers (i.e., 191 pairs) were interested in and participated in this study for one year. Caregivers provided informed assent, while mothers provided informed consent for themselves and their children. The children were 43.9% female and ranged in age from 6.9 to 17.4 months (Mean = 11.8, SD = 3.58) at the beginning of the study. This study was approved by the research ethics committee of in Taiwan, and informed consent was obtained prior to participating. All data were obtained according to Kungtien General Hospital institutional review board (IRB) approved protocols (IRB# 007- 05; title “Teaching strategies and young children's development of effortful control”).

Assessments

Dimensions of child temperament: At the beginning of the study, the three dimensions of child temperament (i.e., negative affectivity, effortful control, and surgency/extraversion) were measured with a standard version of the revised Infant Behavior Questionnaire (IBQ-R), designed to assess temperament for children under age of 18 months. This shortened version has 37 liker-type items and has been shown to have well-established internal consistency, validity, and test-retest reliability [29]. Both mothers and day-time caregivers completed this questionnaire in traditional Chinese at the beginning of the study. The scales in the questionnaires provided acceptable reliabilities (Cronbach’s α) for both mothers and day-time caregivers. The items measuring effortful control included those related to inhibitory control, attention control, perceptual sensitivity, and low intensity pleasure (α mothers=.86; α day-time caregivers= .85). The items assessing negative affectivity included those related to fear, sadness, discomfort, anger/frustration, and difficulty for soothability (α mothers=.83; α day-time caregivers=.78). The surgency/extraversion items included the ones measuring impulsivity, activity level, high intensity pleasure, and low on shyness (α mothers=.83; α day-time caregivers =.86)

Supportive reactions to children's negative emotions: One year later, a measure developed by Stansbury & Sigman (2000) was used to assess adults’ supportive reactions. This measure assessed two types of supportive reactions: a) emotion-focused (meant to comfort children), and b) problem-focused (meant to lead children through cognitive reappraisals by suggesting how the child might re-interpret the situation). Both mothers and day-time caregivers were asked about 6 situations in which children typically experience distress and negative affect 1) when the child was experiencing separation anxiety; 2) when the child was stopped when trying to put an inappropriate item into their mouths; 3) when the child was asked to play alone while mother/ caregiver was busy, 4) when his/her favorite toy/transitional object disappears, 5) when the child was asked to put the toys away while he/she was enjoy playing with them; 6) when the child was asked to eat some food that he/she doesn’t like. For each situation, mothers and day-time caregivers were asked to indicate how often they would provide each type of supportive reactions (i.e., emotion- and problem-focused supportive reactions). Two statements (each representing a type of supportive reactions) were used for each of the scenarios. For each scenario, the participants were asked to rate how often (on a scale ranging from always to never) for both statements.

The scales in the questionnaires provided acceptable reliabilities (Cronbach’s α) for both mothers and day-time caregivers. The items measuring emotion-focused supportive reactions included those related to strategies that help the child feel better (i.e., comforting the child physically or verbally, for example, sings to child or rubs child's back to comfort the child and try to make him/her feel better (α mothers=.71; α day-time caregivers = .81). The items assessing problem-focused supportive reactions included those related to strategies that encourage the child to cope with the problem or to reinterpret the salient features of a frustrating or negative emotional event, especially those that initially elicited negative emotion, in a more neutral or positive manner α mothers=.86; α day-time caregivers= .83).

Data Analyses

We evaluated whether each of the three temperamental dimensions of young children predicts the emotion- and problem-focused supportive reactions of mothers and day-time caregivers with four multiple regression analyses. Each of the multiple regression analysis helps one understand how the level of the dependent variable (i.e., a specific type of supportive reactions) changes when any one of the independent variables (i.e., each of the three dimensions of child temperament - effortful control, negative affectivity, and surgency/extraversion) is varied, while the other independent variables are held fixed.

RESULTS

We first examined the effect of the dimensions of child temperament on emotion-focused supportive behaviors. Effortful control was found to positively predict emotion-focused supportive reactions in both day-time caregiver (β=.18, p=.04) and mothers (β=.22, p=.02). Negative affectivity did not predict emotion-focused supportive reactions in both day-time caregivers (β=.02, p=.79) and mothers (β=.03, p=.71). Surgency/ extraversion also did not predict emotion-focused supportive reactions in both day-time caregivers (β=-.01, p=.94) and caregivers (β=.07, p=.44). Taken together, the three dimensions of child temperament have significant effects on mother’s emotion-focused supportive reactions (R2 =.07, p<.05).

We then examined the effect of the dimensions of child temperament on problem-focused supportive behaviors. Effortful control was found to positively predict problem-focused supportive reactions in both day-time caregivers (β=.30, p<.01) and mothers (β=.32, p<.01). Negative affectivity did negatively predict problem-focused supportive reactions in day-time caregivers (β=-.14, p<.05), but not in mothers (β=-.06, p=.48). Surgency/extraversion also did not predict problem-focused supportive reactions in both day-time caregivers (β=-.07, p=.38) and mothers (β=.13, p=.14). Taken together, the three dimensions of child temperament significantly predict problem-focused supportive reactions in both day-time caregivers and mothers (R2 =.11, p<.01, and R2 =.15, p<.01, respectively).

Table 1: Regressions: Predictors of supportive reactions of mothers and day-time caregivers

  Emotion-focused supportive reactions as dependent variables Problem-focused supportive reactions as dependent variables
Predictors β of day-time caregivers β of mothers’ β of day-time caregivers’ β of mothers’
Effortful control .18* .22* .30** .32**
Negative affectivity 0.02 0.03 -.14* -0.06
Surgency/ extraversion -0.01 0.07 -0.07 0.13
0.03 0.07 0.11 0.15
* p < .05; **p < .01
DISCUSSION

All in all, the results of this study provide support for the study’s proposition that child temperament is associated with certain supportive reactions of both primary caregivers. The purpose of this study was to examine whether child temperamental traits have effects on supportive emotion regulation reactions (i.e., emotional and cognitive responses) of mothers and day-time caregivers. The study results show the effects of child temperamental dispositions on adults’ differential supportive socialization behaviors Thus, we recommend caregivers to always maintain supportive reactions and be aware that they should not be discouraged even if the impact on child's regulation is not immediate.

As effortful control is a dimension of temperament related to the self-regulation of emotional reactivity and behavior, parents and other caregivers need to be aware of children’s individual differences in the ability to voluntarily organize attention, detect errors, and activate a subdominant response in place of a more dominant response [3,24]. Previous studies have shown that effortful control is associated with high responsiveness and positive guidance of parents/caregivers [14,35]. Nonetheless, the direction of effects underlying these associations is noteworthy. Children with higher effortful control are able to voluntarily control their motivation, attention, and actions and thus make their parents/caregivers more likely to feel efficient in assisting their children. However, if a parent/caregiver is aware that the child has poor effortful control, he/she may be more likely to use directive commands rather than supportive strategies [36,37]. As the result of this study shows, mothers/caregivers supportive reactions decrease as effortful control of the children was low in infancy. Therefore, parents/caregivers need to avoid non-supportive reactions while interacting with children who display low effortful control or emotion regulation.

However, the effect of negative affectivity on adults’ supportive reactions was specific to day-time caregivers’ problem-focused support, and did not have effects on day-time caregivers’ emotion-focused support as well as mothers’ problem- and emotion-focused support. The fact that children’s propensity towards negative emotions predicted day-time caregivers’ (but not mothers’) cognitive supports may reflect one of two possibilities. First, it is possible that maternal bonding to the children since their birth might facilitate the emotional relationship between them and also make the cognitive supports about coping strategies different from those between children and non-parent caregivers. Secondly, mothers and children are related to a greater extent than caregivers and children. Evolution has led humans to invest more energy in their descendant offspring. Hence, mothers are likely more patient and resilient with their children such that negative affectivity does not inhibit their providing problem-focused supportive reactions.

One limitation of this study is that it relied on self-report assessments of mothers and caregivers. However, while this may be a limitation, there are also advantages to this procedure. Because of the variable nature of behavior and limitations in motor and verbal proficiency in this age range, the problem of clinical/ observational assessment of child temperament is particularly acute in young children. The structured nature of the typical individual assessment by researchers may limit opportunities for observing effortful control and other characteristics in infants and toddlers. Thus, the primary caregivers possess a wealth of information about the child’s behavior in everyday environment. Nevertheless, future research should use not only questionnaires but also observations to assess adults’ supportive reactions.

Another limitation of this study is the fact that child temperament assessments were collected one year prior to adults’ self-behavior assessments. Although it is possible that developmental shifts could have occurred in the children over the one year period, we do not expect it to be a major problem, because child temperament have been shown to be stable over time [38]. This may also be viewed as a strength of the study, because children’s temperament were conducted early in infancy, rather than later when an individual’s socialized actions are viewed as personality traits [39]. However, future research should try to incorporate a more continuous measure of child and adult assessments. In particular, time lag designs could be used for this purpose.

Moreover, the present study did not administered a prospective design, thus direct relationships between children’s characteristics and caregivers’ supportive reactions coud not be revealed. Future work can jointly examine the effects of children’s characterist and caregivers’ supportive reactions at two time points in a single model, which we were unable to do given the lack of sufficient data. As the overall sample subjects of this study are Taiwanese, the findings of present study may not extend to other ethnic groups. Our results should also be considered specific to the developmental period of infancy and toddlerhood, and do not extend to other developmental stages. Future research needs to take parenting style into account, as it may has an impact on both child's temperament and caregivers' supportive reactions.

The collective effect sizes (R2 ) of the predictor variables on the outcomes may not have been particularly large, although most of the effects were significant. However, given that this study is a preliminary attempt to study child-to-adult effect, and that there is scope to improve measurement and theory in this scheme of research, the significant effect sizes should be considered as important, even if relatively modest in magnitude.

CONCLUSION

Previous studies show that caregivers’ support has effect on child behavior, but our study illustrates that the reverse is also true. The results of this study show that temperamental characteristics that conserve adults’ energy and boost their self-efficacy increase supportive behaviors. Infants who tend to react to environmental demands with self-control (i.e., effortful control) were found to make it easier for both mothers and day-time caregivers to manage and to provide both comforting and cognitive support. However, infants have less effortful self-control hinder adults’ supportive reactions and receive less comforting and cognitive supports.

In line with the previous research that found an association between children’s poor regulation characteristics and parents’ hostility and low quality social interactions [40], this study highlights the effects of children’s temperamental characteristics on adults’ supportive reactions in early life. Previous research has shown that supportive reactions to children’s negative emotions facilitate children’s attempts to constructively regulate emotion and to learn about the needs of others in emotion-evoking situations [10,11,16]. Caregivers need to be aware that they should not be discouraged even if their influence on child's regulation is not immediate. Moreover, if primary caregivers do have the knowledge that some aspects of the children’s temperament can have effects on their supportive reactions, they can control or direct their caring behaviors in a more efficient and productive manner. This has implications for both parents and programs focusing on child development. Therefore, it is important that related programs of caregiver training and parent education should point out the need of increasing tolerance when interacting with children with low effortful control and/or high negative affectivity.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENT

This study was supported by MOST103-2410-H-241-001 from the Ministry of Science and Technology (MOST) of Taiwan. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of MOST. The corresponding author takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. The ideas and analyses presented in this manuscript were not disseminated prior to publication. The authors thank all participating families and caregivers for making this work possible. We are also very grateful to three anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments and peer-review.

DISCLOSURE

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

REFERENCES

1. Brophy-Herb HE, Zajicek-Farber M, McKelvey L, Bocknek E, Stansbury K. Longitudinal connections of maternal supportiveness and early emotion regulation to later school readiness of young children in low-income families. J Soc Social Work Res. 2013; 4: 2-19.

2. Mirabile SP, Scaramella LV, Sohr-Preston SL, Robison SD. Mothers’ socialization of emotion regulation: The moderating role of children’s negative emotional reactivity. Child Youth Care Forum. 2009; 38: 19- 37.

3. Rothbart M, Bates J. Temperament. In: W. Damon, Richard LM, Eisenberg N, editors. Handbook of child psychology: Social, emotional, and personality development. 3. 6th ed. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. 2006; 99-166.

4. Morris AS, Silk JS, Steinberg L, Myers SS, Robinson LR. The role of the family context in the development of emotion regulation. Soc Dev. 2007; 16: 361-388.

5. Galinsky E. Mind in the Making: The Seven Essential Life Skills Every Child Needs. NAEYC special ed. New York: HarperCollins; 2010.

6. Denham SA. Emotional Development in Young Children. New York: Guilford; 1998.

7. Trentacosta CJ, Izard CE. Kindergarten children's emotion competence as a predictor of their academic competence in first grade. Emotion. 2007; 7: 77-88.

8. Eisenberg N, Spinrad TL. Emotion-related regulation: sharpening the definition. Child Dev. 2004; 75: 334-339.

9. Calkins SD, Johnson MC. Toddler regulation of distress to frustrating events: temperamental and maternal correlates. Infant Behav Dev. 1998; 21: 379-395.

10. Eisenberg N, Fabes RA. Mothers' reactions to children's negative emotions: Relations to children's temperament and anger behavior. Merrill Palmer Q. 1994; 40: 138-156.

11. Eisenberg N, Fabes RA, Murphy BC. Parents' Reactions to children's negative emotions: Relations to children's social competence and comforting behavior. Child Dev. 1996; 67: 2227-2247.

12. Gottman JM, Katz LF, Hooven C. Meta-emotion: How families communicate emotionally. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1997.

13. Eisenberg N, Morris AS. Children’s emotion-related regulation. Adv Child Dev Behav. 2002; 30: 189-229.

14. Kochanska G, Murray KT. Mother-child mutually responsive orientation and conscience development: from toddler to early school age. Child Dev. 2000; 71: 417-431.

15. Thompson RA, Meyer S. Socialization of emotion regulation in the family. In: Gross JJ, editor. Handbook of emotion regulation. New York: The Guilford Press. 2007; 249–268.

16. Garner PW, Jones DC, Miner JL. Social competence among low-income preschoolers: emotion socialization practices and social cognitive correlates. Child Dev. 1994; 65: 622-637.

17. Parritz RH. A descriptive analysis of toddler coping in challenging circumstances. Infant Behav Dev. 1996; 19: 171-180.

18. Compas BE, Connor-Smith JK, Saltzman H, Thomsen AH, Wadsworth ME. Coping with stress during childhood and adolescence: problems, progress, and potential in theory and research. Psychol Bull. 2001; 127: 87-127.

19. Barrett K, Campos J. Perspectives on emotional development II: A functionalist approach to emotions. In: Osofsky J, editor. Handbook of infant development. 2nd ed. New York: Wiley. 1987; 555-578.

20. Stansbury K, Sigman M. Responses of preschoolers in two frustrating episodes: emergence of complex strategies for emotion regulation. J Genet Psychol. 2000; 161: 182-202.

21. Cole PM, Dennis TA, Smith-Simon KE, Cohen LH. Preschoolers' emotion regulation strategy understanding: Relations with emotion socialization and child self-regulation. Soc Dev. 2009; 18: 324-352.

22. Fox NA, Henderson HA, Marshall PJ, Nichols KE, Ghera MM. Behavioral inhibition: linking biology and behavior within a developmental framework. Annu Rev Psychol. 2005; 56: 235-262.

23. Kochanska, G. A, N., , Joy ME. Children’s fearfulness as a moderator of parenting in early socialization: Two longitudinal studies. Dev Psychol. 2007; 43: 222-237.

24. Rothbart MK, Derryberry D. Temperament in children. In: C. von Hofsten, Backman L, editors. Psychology at the turn of the millennium: Social, developmental, and clinical perspectives. 2. East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press. 2002; 17-35.

25. Rothbart MK, Sheese BE. Temperament and emotion regulation. In: Gross JJ, editor. Handbook of emotion regulation. New York: Guilford Press. 2007; 331-350.

26. Bridgett DJ, Gartstein MA, Putnam SP, McKay T, Iddins E, Robertson C, et al. Maternal and contextual influences and the effect of temperament development during infancy on parenting in toddlerhood. Infant Behav Dev. 2009; 32: 103-116.

27. Lengua LJ. Growth in temperament and parenting as predictors of adjustment during children's transition to adolescence. Dev Psychol. 2006; 42: 819-832.

28. Pettit GS, Keiley MK, Laird RD, Bates JE, Dodge KA. Predicting the developmental course of mother-reported monitoring across childhood and adolescence from early proactive parenting, child temperament, and parents' worries. J Fam Psychol. 2007; 21: 206- 217.

29. Gartstein MA, Rothbart MK. Studying infant temperament via the revised infant behavior questionnaire. Infant Behav Dev. 2003; 26: 64-86.

30. Scaramella LV, Leve LD. Clarifying parent-child reciprocities during early childhood: the early childhood coercion model. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2004; 7: 89-107.

31. Calkins SD. Does aversive behavior during toddlerhood matter? The effects of difficult temperament on maternal perceptions and behavior. Infant Ment Health J. 2002; 23: 381-402.

32. Eisenberg N, Fabes RA, Shepard SA, Guthrie IK, Murphy BC, Reiser M. Parental reactions to children's negative emotions: longitudinal relations to quality of children's social functioning. Child Dev. 1999; 70: 513-534.

33. Mertesacker B, Bade U, Haverkock A, Pauli-Pott U. Predicting maternal reactivity/sensitivity: The role of infant emotionality, maternal depressiveness/anxiety, and social support. Infant Ment Health J. 2004; 25: 47-61.

34. Rothbart MK, Putnam SP. Temperament and socialization. In: Pulkkinen L, Caspi A, editors. Paths to successful development: Personality in the life course. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. 2002; 19-45.

35. Lunkenheimer ES, Dishion TJ, Shaw DS, Connell AM, Gardner F, Wilson MN, et al. Collateral benefits of the Family Check-Up on early childhood school readiness: indirect effects of parents' positive behavior support. Dev Psychol. 2008; 44: 1737-1752.

36. Eisenberg N, Vidmar M, Spinrad TL, Eggum ND, Edwards A, Gaertner B, et al. Mothers' teaching strategies and children's effortful control: a longitudinal study. Dev Psychol. 2010; 46: 1294-1308.

37. Li I, Pawan C, Stansbury K. Emerging effortful control in infancy and toddlerhood and maternal support: A child driven or parent driven model? Infant Behav Dev. 2014; 37: 216-224.

38. Rimm-Kaufman SE, Kagan J. Infant predictors of kindergarten behavior: The contribution of inhibited and uninhibited temperament types. Behav Disorders. 2005; 30: 329-345.

39. Kagan J, Fox NA. Biology, culture, and temperamental biases. In: Damon W, Lerner R, Eisenberg N, editors. Handbook of child psychology: Social, emotional, and personality development. 3. 6th ed. New York: Wiley. 2006; 99-166.

40. Brody GH, Ge X. Linking parenting processes and self-regulation to psychological functioning and alcohol use during early adolescence. J Fam Psychol. 2001; 15: 82-94.

About the Corresponding Author

Dr. Ishien Li
Summary of background:

My graduate training in developmental psychology and early childhood education focused on typical social-emotional development, while my postdoctoral research interests focus primarily on children’s individual differences in temperament/predispositions and how these differences affect their social-emotional development and their interaction with parents and teachers. These interests extend to determining how parenting moderates genetic and environmental influences on child behavior (i.e., gene-environment interaction).

Permanent e-mail address: liishien@gmail.com

Current research focus:

  • Examining within- and across-time relations between children’s temperament and adults' supportive reactions with structural equation models to understand the contributions of children's temperament to their social interactions.
  • Understanding the etiology of antisocial behaviors across childhood and adolescence with twin studies to understand the role of genotype-environment interplay.
  • Obtaining a better understanding of the environmental, genetic and neurobiological factors that influence children's psychological functioning and behavior.

Chang CH, Li I (2017) Effects of children’s temperament on mothers’ and caregivers’ supportive reactions related to socialization of emotion regulation. J Behav 2(1): 1007

Received : 28 Jan 2017
Accepted : 23 Mar 2017
Published : 13 Apr 2017
Journals
Annals of Otolaryngology and Rhinology
ISSN : 2379-948X
Launched : 2014
JSM Schizophrenia
Launched : 2016
Journal of Nausea
Launched : 2020
JSM Internal Medicine
Launched : 2016
JSM Hepatitis
Launched : 2016
JSM Oro Facial Surgeries
ISSN : 2578-3211
Launched : 2016
Journal of Human Nutrition and Food Science
ISSN : 2333-6706
Launched : 2013
JSM Regenerative Medicine and Bioengineering
ISSN : 2379-0490
Launched : 2013
JSM Spine
ISSN : 2578-3181
Launched : 2016
Archives of Palliative Care
ISSN : 2573-1165
Launched : 2016
JSM Nutritional Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3203
Launched : 2017
Annals of Neurodegenerative Disorders
ISSN : 2476-2032
Launched : 2016
Journal of Fever
ISSN : 2641-7782
Launched : 2017
JSM Bone Marrow Research
ISSN : 2578-3351
Launched : 2016
JSM Mathematics and Statistics
ISSN : 2578-3173
Launched : 2014
Journal of Autoimmunity and Research
ISSN : 2573-1173
Launched : 2014
JSM Arthritis
ISSN : 2475-9155
Launched : 2016
JSM Head and Neck Cancer-Cases and Reviews
ISSN : 2573-1610
Launched : 2016
JSM General Surgery Cases and Images
ISSN : 2573-1564
Launched : 2016
JSM Anatomy and Physiology
ISSN : 2573-1262
Launched : 2016
JSM Dental Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1548
Launched : 2016
Annals of Emergency Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1017
Launched : 2016
Annals of Mens Health and Wellness
ISSN : 2641-7707
Launched : 2017
Journal of Preventive Medicine and Health Care
ISSN : 2576-0084
Launched : 2018
Journal of Chronic Diseases and Management
ISSN : 2573-1300
Launched : 2016
Annals of Vaccines and Immunization
ISSN : 2378-9379
Launched : 2014
JSM Heart Surgery Cases and Images
ISSN : 2578-3157
Launched : 2016
Annals of Reproductive Medicine and Treatment
ISSN : 2573-1092
Launched : 2016
JSM Brain Science
ISSN : 2573-1289
Launched : 2016
JSM Biomarkers
ISSN : 2578-3815
Launched : 2014
JSM Biology
ISSN : 2475-9392
Launched : 2016
Archives of Stem Cell and Research
ISSN : 2578-3580
Launched : 2014
Annals of Clinical and Medical Microbiology
ISSN : 2578-3629
Launched : 2014
JSM Pediatric Surgery
ISSN : 2578-3149
Launched : 2017
Journal of Memory Disorder and Rehabilitation
ISSN : 2578-319X
Launched : 2016
JSM Tropical Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2578-3165
Launched : 2016
JSM Head and Face Medicine
ISSN : 2578-3793
Launched : 2016
JSM Cardiothoracic Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1297
Launched : 2016
JSM Bone and Joint Diseases
ISSN : 2578-3351
Launched : 2017
JSM Bioavailability and Bioequivalence
ISSN : 2641-7812
Launched : 2017
JSM Atherosclerosis
ISSN : 2573-1270
Launched : 2016
Journal of Genitourinary Disorders
ISSN : 2641-7790
Launched : 2017
Journal of Fractures and Sprains
ISSN : 2578-3831
Launched : 2016
Journal of Autism and Epilepsy
ISSN : 2641-7774
Launched : 2016
Annals of Marine Biology and Research
ISSN : 2573-105X
Launched : 2014
JSM Health Education & Primary Health Care
ISSN : 2578-3777
Launched : 2016
JSM Communication Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3807
Launched : 2016
Annals of Musculoskeletal Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3599
Launched : 2016
Annals of Virology and Research
ISSN : 2573-1122
Launched : 2014
JSM Renal Medicine
ISSN : 2573-1637
Launched : 2016
Journal of Muscle Health
ISSN : 2578-3823
Launched : 2016
JSM Genetics and Genomics
ISSN : 2334-1823
Launched : 2013
JSM Anxiety and Depression
ISSN : 2475-9139
Launched : 2016
Clinical Journal of Heart Diseases
ISSN : 2641-7766
Launched : 2016
Annals of Medicinal Chemistry and Research
ISSN : 2378-9336
Launched : 2014
JSM Pain and Management
ISSN : 2578-3378
Launched : 2016
JSM Women's Health
ISSN : 2578-3696
Launched : 2016
Clinical Research in HIV or AIDS
ISSN : 2374-0094
Launched : 2013
Journal of Endocrinology, Diabetes and Obesity
ISSN : 2333-6692
Launched : 2013
Journal of Substance Abuse and Alcoholism
ISSN : 2373-9363
Launched : 2013
JSM Neurosurgery and Spine
ISSN : 2373-9479
Launched : 2013
Journal of Liver and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2379-0830
Launched : 2014
Journal of Drug Design and Research
ISSN : 2379-089X
Launched : 2014
JSM Clinical Oncology and Research
ISSN : 2373-938X
Launched : 2013
JSM Bioinformatics, Genomics and Proteomics
ISSN : 2576-1102
Launched : 2014
JSM Chemistry
ISSN : 2334-1831
Launched : 2013
Journal of Trauma and Care
ISSN : 2573-1246
Launched : 2014
JSM Surgical Oncology and Research
ISSN : 2578-3688
Launched : 2016
Annals of Food Processing and Preservation
ISSN : 2573-1033
Launched : 2016
Journal of Radiology and Radiation Therapy
ISSN : 2333-7095
Launched : 2013
JSM Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation
ISSN : 2578-3572
Launched : 2016
Annals of Clinical Pathology
ISSN : 2373-9282
Launched : 2013
Annals of Cardiovascular Diseases
ISSN : 2641-7731
Launched : 2016
Annals of Clinical and Experimental Metabolism
ISSN : 2572-2492
Launched : 2016
Clinical Research in Infectious Diseases
ISSN : 2379-0636
Launched : 2013
JSM Microbiology
ISSN : 2333-6455
Launched : 2013
Journal of Urology and Research
ISSN : 2379-951X
Launched : 2014
Journal of Family Medicine and Community Health
ISSN : 2379-0547
Launched : 2013
Annals of Pregnancy and Care
ISSN : 2578-336X
Launched : 2017
JSM Cell and Developmental Biology
ISSN : 2379-061X
Launched : 2013
Annals of Aquaculture and Research
ISSN : 2379-0881
Launched : 2014
Clinical Research in Pulmonology
ISSN : 2333-6625
Launched : 2013
Journal of Immunology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2333-6714
Launched : 2013
Annals of Forensic Research and Analysis
ISSN : 2378-9476
Launched : 2014
JSM Biochemistry and Molecular Biology
ISSN : 2333-7109
Launched : 2013
Annals of Breast Cancer Research
ISSN : 2641-7685
Launched : 2016
Annals of Gerontology and Geriatric Research
ISSN : 2378-9409
Launched : 2014
Journal of Sleep Medicine and Disorders
ISSN : 2379-0822
Launched : 2014
JSM Burns and Trauma
ISSN : 2475-9406
Launched : 2016
Chemical Engineering and Process Techniques
ISSN : 2333-6633
Launched : 2013
Annals of Clinical Cytology and Pathology
ISSN : 2475-9430
Launched : 2014
JSM Allergy and Asthma
ISSN : 2573-1254
Launched : 2016
Journal of Neurological Disorders and Stroke
ISSN : 2334-2307
Launched : 2013
Annals of Sports Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2379-0571
Launched : 2014
JSM Sexual Medicine
ISSN : 2578-3718
Launched : 2016
Annals of Vascular Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2378-9344
Launched : 2014
JSM Biotechnology and Biomedical Engineering
ISSN : 2333-7117
Launched : 2013
Journal of Hematology and Transfusion
ISSN : 2333-6684
Launched : 2013
JSM Environmental Science and Ecology
ISSN : 2333-7141
Launched : 2013
Journal of Cardiology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2333-6676
Launched : 2013
JSM Nanotechnology and Nanomedicine
ISSN : 2334-1815
Launched : 2013
Journal of Ear, Nose and Throat Disorders
ISSN : 2475-9473
Launched : 2016
JSM Ophthalmology
ISSN : 2333-6447
Launched : 2013
Journal of Pharmacology and Clinical Toxicology
ISSN : 2333-7079
Launched : 2013
Annals of Psychiatry and Mental Health
ISSN : 2374-0124
Launched : 2013
Medical Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology
ISSN : 2333-6439
Launched : 2013
Annals of Pediatrics and Child Health
ISSN : 2373-9312
Launched : 2013
JSM Clinical Pharmaceutics
ISSN : 2379-9498
Launched : 2014
JSM Foot and Ankle
ISSN : 2475-9112
Launched : 2016
JSM Alzheimer's Disease and Related Dementia
ISSN : 2378-9565
Launched : 2014
Journal of Addiction Medicine and Therapy
ISSN : 2333-665X
Launched : 2013
Journal of Veterinary Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2378-931X
Launched : 2013
Annals of Public Health and Research
ISSN : 2378-9328
Launched : 2014
Annals of Orthopedics and Rheumatology
ISSN : 2373-9290
Launched : 2013
Journal of Clinical Nephrology and Research
ISSN : 2379-0652
Launched : 2014
Annals of Community Medicine and Practice
ISSN : 2475-9465
Launched : 2014
Annals of Biometrics and Biostatistics
ISSN : 2374-0116
Launched : 2013
JSM Clinical Case Reports
ISSN : 2373-9819
Launched : 2013
Journal of Cancer Biology and Research
ISSN : 2373-9436
Launched : 2013
Journal of Surgery and Transplantation Science
ISSN : 2379-0911
Launched : 2013
Journal of Dermatology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2373-9371
Launched : 2013
JSM Gastroenterology and Hepatology
ISSN : 2373-9487
Launched : 2013
Annals of Nursing and Practice
ISSN : 2379-9501
Launched : 2014
JSM Dentistry
ISSN : 2333-7133
Launched : 2013
Author Information X