Loading

Journal of Behavior

Refining the Construct of 'Anger' in Relation to Personality Disorders

Review Article | Open Access | Volume 2 | Issue 3

  • 1. Institute of Mental Health, Nottingham, UK
+ Show More - Show Less
Corresponding Authors
Richard C. Howard, Nerobergstrasse 22, 65193 Wiesbaden, Germany, Tel: +44-611-52-7264
Abstract

This paper highlights the importance of anger problems in psychopathology generally, and in personality disorders in particular. It argues, firstly, that problematic anger transcends traditional diagnostic boundaries and can best be viewed through the prism of the recently proposed hierarchical taxonomy of psychopathology; secondly, that anger is a heterogeneous construct that can be experienced as hedonically positive (experienced as pleasant: ‘appetitive anger’) as well as negative (experienced as unpleasant: ‘aversive anger’). Four distinct types of anger are proposed in the context of the author's quadripartite violence typology: thrill seeking and coercive (both subtypes of appetitive anger) and explosive/reactive and vengeful/ruminative (both subtypes of aversive anger). Evidence is reviewed that supports this anger typology in studies of delinquent youth. Finally, the triggers of anger are considered in relation to the 4 types of anger. It is suggested that personality disorders differ not only with regard to the type of anger with which they are associated, but also with regard to the triggers that elicit anger.

Keywords
  • Emotion
  • Personality disorder
  • Anger
  • Aggression
  • Violence
Citation

Howard R (2017) Refining the Construct of 'Anger' in Relation to Personality Disorders. J Behav 2(3): 1013

INTRODUCTION

Anger is a normal experience and a common one for the majority of people [1]. Indeed, anger is regarded among theorists of emotion as one of the 'core' or primary emotions [2,3]. Anger only becomes a problem for individuals when they are impaired in their ability to self-regulate and control their angry experiences, thoughts and impulses. When thus impaired, individuals are at increased risk of engaging in criminal behavior, and in aggression and violence in particular, notwithstanding that aggression and violence can occur without anger being an antecedent [4]. According to this view, anger, at least when experienced as an unpleasant affect, is neither a necessary nor a sufficient condition for aggression or violence to occur. However, in contrast with the view that anger is always experienced as unpleasant (i.e. hedonically negative), it has been argued, in the context of the author’s proposed quadripartite violence typology [5,6], that anger associated with violence in people with personality disorders (PDs) can be experienced as either unpleasant (hedonically negative) or as pleasant (hedonically positive). A central purpose of this article is to explore in greater depth the idea that anger in the context of different types of violence perpetrated by personality disordered individuals can be experienced either as pleasant or as unpleasant, depending on the motive for their violence: thrill-seeking and self-gratification on the one hand, or self-protection and revenge on the other hand: see Figure 1. Pleasant anger will be referred to here as ‘appetitive anger’, while unpleasant anger will be referred to here as ‘aversive anger’ (see section 3 below).

As well as describing four different types of violence, Figure 1 describes a two dimensional affective/motivational space defined by the interaction of dimensions of affective valence (positive vs. negative) and impulsiveness (impulsive vs. controlled). This yields four distinct types of emotional state: excitement/ exhilaration (associated with the upper left quadrant in Figure 1); fear/distress (associated with the upper right quadrant in Figure 1); vengefulness (associated with the lower right quadrant in Figure 1); and pleasurable feelings of anticipation (associated with the lower left quadrant in 

1 . In an earlier description of the model [6] it was emphasized that movement is possible across quadrants within this affective/motivational space, which is dynamic rather than static. For example, sudden shifts in emotional state may occur in the course of a single violent episode: “One can easily see how, in rare cases where someone runs amok and carries out a series of killings, the episode might originate in a desire to wreak revenge for some perceived wrong… but once the violence has started, the perpetrator, viewing the whole episode through a protective frame and feeling excited and empowered by it, might have switched into a motivational state where he seeks ever higher degrees of arousal, culminating even in his own self-destruction, the ultimate act of excitement seeking” [6, p. 8)]

1. Anger in Personality Disorder

Linking anger problems to psychopathology in general, and to PD in particular, is problematic; firstly because the anger construct itself is heterogeneous (this is discussed further in section 3 below), and secondly because anger problems transcend specific psychiatric diagnoses. Excessive anger, both in regard to its experience as an affective state and its outward expression, is central to the diagnosis of several disorders listed in the American Psychiatric Association's (APA) 5th edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM-5) [7]: Intermittent Explosive Disorder, Borderline Personality Disorder (BPD), Bipolar Disorder, Disruptive Mood Dysregulation, and Oppositional Defiant Disorder (reviewed in [9]). While excessive anger is explicitly linked in DSM-IV/5 only to borderline personality disorder (e.g. 'inappropriate intense anger or difficulty controlling anger, e.g. frequent displays of temper, constant anger, recurring physical fights'), it is implicitly or indirectly linked to three additional PDs: antisocial, narcissistic and paranoid [4]. The co-occurrence of two or more PDs ('co-morbidity') is the rule rather than the exception among personality disordered patients [10]. This is particularly the case in forensic contexts where the frequent co-occurrence of antisocial and borderline PDs represents a toxic concatenation of maladaptive personality traits that is associated with high levels of experienced and expressed anger, both among PD patients residing in the community [11,12] and among PD patients detained in high-secure forensic hospital, in whom high levels of anger were significantly associated with a greater incidence of institutional aggression [13].

PDs are typified by relatively2 enduring, inflexible, and pervasive disturbances in how individuals experience and interpret themselves, others, and the world around them. These are reflected in maladaptive patterns of cognition, emotion, and behaviour that result in significant problems in psychosocial functioning, particularly in interpersonal relationships. As a consequence, people suffering from PD are at high risk of coming into contact with the criminal justice system and, particularly when the PD is severe, of being incarcerated in prisons and high-security psychiatric hospitals (see [16] for a review of PD and offending). Maladaptive traits associated with PD, in particular traits related to externalizing, thought disorder and psychopathy, are thought to link PD to violence and violent offending [17, 18].

Footnotes:
¹ These emotional states bear some resemblance to several of the seven primary emotions proposed by Panksepp (discussed further below), e.g. his SEEKING/desire system is associated with excitement/exhiliration and, when overactive, “can promote excessive and impulsive behaviors“ [8, p.538]. Unlike the conceptualization of anger proposed here, in Panksepp’s analysis the experience of anger is associated explicitly and exclusively with the RAGE system. According to Panksepp, anger is 
affectively negative and triggered when SEEKING/desire is frustrated: “When SEEKING is thwarted, RAGE is aroused” [8, p.538]. 
² The relative stability of PD features is emphasised here, since not only do PD symptoms show considerable within-individual variability from day to day [14], they also show significant decline across years of follow-up [15]. 
2. Anger Within An Hierarchical Taxonomy Of Psychopathology

In reviewing anger and PD, Howells [4] pointed out that anger has been neglected as a topic in relation to PDs. He stated (p. 204): "Anger appears to be too important a problem in people with PDs for it to remain unaddressed...Anger has many environmental, cognitive, affective, psychophysiological and self-regulatory components and the patterns of such components for particular PDs need to be established." Notwithstanding this plea for an examination of anger components in relation to particular PDs, one reason for the neglect of anger in relation to PD, particularly in the clinical literature, is an excessive focus on distinct categories of PD and an acceptance of these categories as existing 'in nature'. Dissatisfaction with the traditional DSM-IV/5 PD categories themselves, on account of their high overlap (the comorbidity problem), their heterogeneous nature and their poorly defined boundaries, has given rise to an analysis of psychopathology in terms of levels within an overall hierarchical organization. For example, the recently proposed HiTOP model of psychopathology [19] posits that psychopathology is hierarchically structured, i.e., symptoms/signs (level 1) are nested within maladaptive traits (level 2) which in turn are nested within syndromes/disorders (level 3). At a higher level of the hierarchy (level 5) are situated broad spectra, namely internalizing pathology, externalizing pathology (comprising disinhibited and antagonistic), thought disorder (i.e., psychosis spectrum disorders), and detachment (i.e., pathological introversion). At the highest level of the hierarchy are super-spectra such as general psychopathology (p). By this means, superordinate domains can account for the interdependence of lower-order structures such as correlated personality traits and psychopathological syndromes. In particular, PD comorbidity can be seen as due to associations between broad liability factors (spectra) rather than to disorder-specific associations [20]. Given that problematic anger occurs across such a broad spectrum of psychopathology, we must ask whether anger problems perhaps reflect a predisposition that occurs transdiagnostically, at the level of spectra. One might expect several spectra in the HiTOP model to be highly relevant to anger problems, in particular the thought disorder, antagonistic externalizing and disinhibited externalizing spectra.

One such transdiagnostic predisposition is what has been referred to as emotional impulsiveness or "Urgency", reflecting the tendency to act rashly and precipitately under pressure of positive or negative emotions (positive and negative Urgency, respectively). Negative Urgency is a core feature of severe personality disorder in mentally disordered offenders [21], and anger is likely to be a key driver of emotionally impulsive behavior in those whose PD is severe, e.g. those with antisocial/ borderline PD comorbidity. A recent meta-analytic review [22] showed that emotional impulsiveness - particularly negative Urgency - extends across a broad spectrum of psychopathology, including both externalizing and internalizing disorders. This suggests that a predisposition to emotional impulsiveness may contribute importantly to a general psychopathology (p) factor or 'super spectrum' associated with more life impairment, general familiality, worse developmental histories, and more compromised early-life brain function [23].

However, anger can also be represented at lower levels within this hierarchy. For example, maladaptive traits associated with anger and aggression can be represented at the level of ‘symptom components and maladaptive traits’ in HiTOP. Recent evidence [24] indicates that two specific DSM-5 (section 3) trait facets, hostility (subsumed under the internalizing spectrum in HiTOP) and risk taking (subsumed under the disinhibited externalizing spectrum in HiTOP), are significantly associated with aggression in offenders, and by implication with anger. At a still lower level in the HiTOP hierarchy ('signs and symptoms'), anger can be represented by anger-related behaviours such as shouting, threatening, attacking, etc. In summary, therefore, problematic anger is arguably embedded in all levels within this hierarchical structure, from (at the lowest level) signs and symptoms to (at the highest level) higher-order dimensions or 'super spectra'.

The dimensions represented at the highest level of the HiTOP hierarchy, that is, at the level of spectra and super spectra, constitute heritable dispositions or 'temperaments' that give rise, at successively later periods of development, to lower-order manifestations such as syndromes/disorders and maladaptive traits. Personality disorders, for example, generally manifest in late adolescence or early adulthood, although they may often manifest in childhood [25]. Recent evidence [26] suggests that early (at age 10-11) dysregulation reflects a temperamental vulnerability that gives rise to personality pathology when children grow older. Results of several recent studies [27,28,29] reveal an important role for problematic anger in childhood and adolescence as a precursor of adult antisocial behaviour, suggesting that emotional impulsiveness may be an important strand that runs continuously through the developmental fabric of severe personality disorder, from early through late childhood into adolescence and adulthood.

Relationships between emotions such as anger and PD at the level of spectra or components have not to date been investigated, reflecting perhaps the damaging separation of theories of PD from mainstream psychological analyses of emotion [4]. Early theorists of emotion such as Plutchik [2] emphasised that emotions were closely related to psychiatric diagnoses. He argued that psychiatric diagnoses represent extremes or exaggerations of personality traits, which in turn represent mixtures of basic emotions. Recent evidence, considered below, is consistent with this, suggesting that emotions are embedded within all PD diagnoses. Likewise, Panksepp’s evolutionary theory posits seven primary emotions: seeking, fear, care, anger, sadness/panic, lust and play [30]. These primary emotions are expressed in individuals as personality traits that reflect both the intensity of the various primary emotions and the ways in which individuals have learned to cope with them. Primary emotions are said to have a distinct brain localization that reflects their evolutionary origins, being located in the brain stem and lower limbic area.

An important study [31] explored relationships between PDs and the primary emotions using the Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales developed by Davis and Panksepp [32]. Anger was defined in this study as feeling hot-headed, being easily irritated and frustrated, experiencing anger verbally or physically and remaining angry for long periods. After accounting for variance shared between the different emotions, anger was found to be significantly associated with a majority of PDs, in particular with paranoid, borderline, antisocial, narcissistic and obsessive-compulsive PDs.

Among most theorists of emotion, including Plutchik [2], anger has been conceptualised as a unitary process of reacting to a stimulus (e.g. a threat) in order to deal with it (e.g. to remove the threat). In contrast, it is argued in the following section that, in the context of PD and violence, the anger construct can be refined and extended to include positive, as well as negative, hedonic states. It will be further argued, in section 4, that individuals with PD are hypersensitive to particular triggers of anger – their thresholds for eliciting anger are reduced vis-à-vis these triggers.

3. Refining The Anger Construct: Appetitive vs. Aversive Anger

A problem with linking anger to psychopathology in general, and PD in particular, is the heterogeneous nature of the anger construct. Anger has been found to be multidimensional, at least in regard to anger-related behaviours, even in non-clinical, community samples. For example, in a native American sample 4 anger types were identified, including 'externalising' and 'internalising' types [33]. The 'externalizing' type acted out their anger by yelling and fighting, while the 'internalizing' type engaged in crying and thoughts of self-harm when angry. An earlier study [34] identified a similar 'internalizing' anger type characterised by keeping feelings inside, feeling emotionally bothered and ruminating about the anger-provoking event. While anger has traditionally been viewed as hedonically negative - indeed it is probably most commonly experienced as unpleasant - it is increasingly recognized that anger can also be experienced as hedonically positive ('pleasant anger'). Evidence from neuroscience suggests that the increased risk of violence conferred on individuals by virtue of their having low brain dopamine activity is mediated by high sensation seeking [35]. This implies that violence can be motivated by a desire for excitement and enhanced positive affect, an idea that is prominently represented in the quadripartite violence typology (QVT) [5,6]. In this typology (see Figure 1)

Figure 1. The quadripartite (2×2) violence typology. The intersection of  impulsiveness (vs. control/premeditation) and affect (positive vs. negative) yields  4 distinct types of violence characterised by motives of excitement seeking and  self-gratification (both associated with positive affect) and revenge and selfprotection (both associated with negative affec

Figure 1. The quadripartite (2×2) violence typology. The intersection of impulsiveness (vs. control/premeditation) and affect (positive vs. negative) yields 4 distinct types of violence characterised by motives of excitement seeking and self-gratification (both associated with positive affect) and revenge and selfprotection (both associated with negative affect

, there are four types of anger, each linked to a particular type of violence. Two of these anger types - thrill-seeking and coercive - are associated with positive affect and can be referred to as 'appetitive anger'. The remaining two anger types - explosive/reactive and vengeful/ruminative - are associated with negative affect and can be referred to as 'aversive anger'. Consistent with anger traditionally being regarded as affectively negative (experienced as unpleasant), the explosive/ reactive and vengeful/ruminative anger types have received most attention in the literature. For example Agnew [36] proposed that anger creates a disposition for violence, fostering the belief that violence is justified (to “right a wrong”) and creating a desire for revenge. Revenge has been reported as a common motive for violence among young female violent offenders [37]. In this study, revenge-motivated violence was classed as 'instrumental', in contrast to QVT where it is classed as controlled/aversive rather than as controlled/appetitive. Thrill-seeking violence, motivated by a quest for excitement [6], is a common phenomenon [38]; it is accompanied by high arousal that is experienced as a pleasant feeling of malicious glee [39], exemplified in the behaviour of the mass murderer Anders Breivik, diagnosed with narcissistic PD with features of antisocial PD [40]. While engaged in a shooting spree on the island of Utøya, Norway, on 22 July 2011, Breivik was reported to have whooped with glee while going about his bloody business. Having once embarked on his shooting spree Breivik likely flipped into an emotional state of high excitement, albeit his murderous impulses were fuelled by an extreme right-wing ideology and a deep-seated sense of grievance at what he perceived as the Islamisation of his country. Coercive anger, also hedonically positive, is said to accompany violence that is instrumental and driven by a desire for self-gratification (material or sexual) at the expense of others. A desire to dominate or coerce others is a feature of this violence type. The final type of anger, called explosive/reactive, accompanies violence that is motivated by the desire to remove an immediately present interpersonal threat, physical or psychological. Here the affect is negative, accompanied by fear and distress.

A self-report questionnaire, the Angry Aggression Scales (AAS) was developed to tap these 4 anger types and has been applied in two studies of delinquent youth with severe conduct problems and a control sample of prosocial youth [41,42]. In the first study [41], the factor structure of the AAS was examined using promax rotation with Kaiser normalization. Based on the eigenvalue greater than one criterion, four factors were extracted. The first factor, corresponding to thrill-seeking anger items (e.g. When I make someone suffer I get “turned on” and lose control), accounted for 52.4% of the variance; the second factor, corresponding to the five coercive anger items (e.g. I sometimes pretend I’m angry to make others afraid of me), accounted for 6.8% of the variance; the third factor, corresponding to the vengeful/ruminative anger items (e.g. If I get really angry at someone I can spend a long time plotting revenge), accounted for 6.1% of the variance; the fourth factor, accounting for 5.5% of the variance, comprised explosive/reactive anger items (e.g. When I get angry I easily lose my self-control). Cronbach’ s alpha values were 0.93 for thrill-seeking, 0.87 for coercive, 0.86 for vengeful/ ruminative, and 0.84 for explosive/reactive. Teacher-rated proactive aggression was positively and significantly associated with both AAS thrill-seeking and coercive scales, while teacher- rated reactive aggression was most strongly associated with the AAS explosive/reactive scale, but also to some degree with the AAS vengeful/ruminative scale.

Results of the second study [43] provided further validation of the proposed anger typology by finding unique associations between anger type and different measures of disruptive conduct. Vengeful/ruminative anger uniquely predicted participants’ cognitive problems and their failure to cooperate as rated by their teachers. Thrill-seeking anger uniquely predicted all forms of self-reported delinquency. Explosive/reactive anger uniquely predicted self-reported expulsion from school and teacher ratings of poor self-control and externalizing behaviour problems. Affective disturbance in youths, as rated by their teachers, was negatively associated with thrillseeking anger and positively associated with explosive/reactive and vengeful/ruminative forms of anger. A further study [43] examined the relationship of the violence types posited by QVT and functions of violence, using a typology developed to assess functions of violence [44]. The function 'express anger', previously identified as the most common function for aggressive acts in personality disordered offenders [45], correlated significantly and positively with both aversively motivated violence types: r = .57 with aversive/ impulsive and r = .28 with aversive/controlled. "Express anger" correlated negatively with both appetitively motivated types of violence: r = -.39 with appetitive/impulsive and r = -.45 with appetitive/controlled. It should be noted that the 'express anger' function identifies anger expressed in the context of negative affect - that is, it does not capture appetitive anger.

4. Triggers Of Anger

Implicit in the typology of anger outlined here is the idea that each type of anger is associated with a particular class of elicitor or trigger. Thus explosive/reactive anger is elicited when the individual is confronted by what is perceived as a direct interpersonal threat to his or her safety; this could take the form of a verbal or physical provocation, or even a provocative look. Appetitive anger is elicited by stimuli that evoke either greed or lust (in the case of coercive anger) or that provide an opportunity for maximizing positive affect (in the case of thrill-seeking anger). Finally, in the case of vengeful/ruminative anger, the evoking stimulus is a verbal slight, an injury or a perceived wrong that evokes feelings of vengeance. This type of anger is typically associated with both narcissistic and borderline PDs [4]. In the former case the eliciting stimulus is typically personal criticism (e.g. a disparaging remark) or threats to the individual's ego; the individual with narcissistic PD is "likely to respond to criticism with rage" [46,p. 114]. In the case of borderline PD the triggers are thought to be rejection or threatened abandonment, real or imagined [9]. A recent study [47] demonstrated that among women with prominent borderline personality disorder symptoms, anger arousal in the context of perceived rejection - but not in response to criticism - was key to the enhanced risk for aggressive urges and behaviors shown by these women in their daily life. Importantly, co-occurring antisocial PD symptoms attenuated the link between perceived rejection and aggression, suggesting that the pathway from rejection to aggression may be unique to borderline PD. The enhanced risk for aggressive urges consequent upon perceived rejection in borderline PD patients likely accounts for their increased contact with the criminal justice system (e.g. [48]). In this study an inability to control emotional impulses was found to mediate the relationship between severity of borderline symptoms and criminal justice contact. While, as shown above, the critical triggers for eliciting anger in borderline and narcissistic patients appear to be, respectively, rejection and an ego-threatening slight, the critical trigger in antisocial PD patients is less clear-cut but might be characterized by general frustration or threats to dominance [4]. However, given that antisocial PD is linked to excitement seeking, we might expect the triggers for anger in antisocial PD patients to include stimuli that provide an opportunity for maximizing positive affect in the form thrill seeking. In the final section below, a possible model is presented showing how rejection in borderline patients might trigger an 'emotional cascade' resulting in a progressively worsening aversive emotional and cognitive experience that the individual finds difficult to terminate.

5. Hostile Rumination And Emotional Cascades

Hostile rumination has been found to be uniquely associated with borderline PD features and to mediate the link between a negative early environment and borderline features in adulthood [49]. Since hostile rumination is associated with a desire to seek revenge in response to situations that have been perceived as threatening [50], it is likely associated with violence motivated by a desire for vengeance (the “appetitive/controlled” subtype shown in Figure 1). The “emotion cascade” model developed by Selby and colleagues [51], illustrated in

Figure 2

Figure 2. An emotional cascade, initiated by rejection, leads to rumination which,  through a positive feedback loop, serves to increase the intensity of the negative  emotion (feelings of vengeance). A progressively worsening aversive emotional and  cognitive experience is generated that the individual finds difficult to terminate.

Figure 2. An emotional cascade, initiated by rejection, leads to rumination which, through a positive feedback loop, serves to increase the intensity of the negative emotion (feelings of vengeance). A progressively worsening aversive emotional and cognitive experience is generated that the individual finds difficult to terminate.

, provides a possible mechanism through which hostile rumination in individuals with borderline PD, triggered by a self-threatening provocation such as a slight or a rejection, might make them more susceptible to a cycle of increasing rumination and negative affect, leading them to commit a retributive act of violence. Empirical support has been provided for the emotional cascade model, with the suggestion that impulsive behaviours might provide physical distraction that short-circuits the emotion cascade by reducing rumination [52]. These authors provided evidence that individuals with borderline PD may be especially susceptible to emotional cascades; they suggest that: “BPD may be a disorder characterized as the extreme end of a continuum of emotional cascades and impulsivity” [52,p. 13]. Confirmation that individuals with borderline PD are indeed particularly susceptible to emotional cascades involving hostile rumination, and that disinhibited behaviours including violence can act to shortcircuit the emotional cascade, would offer an important insight into the underlying motivational dynamics of violence in borderline PD.

6. Implications And Conclusion

Anger is an important but heterogeneous construct; problematic anger transcends particular psychiatric diagnoses and is particularly relevant to PD. Assessment of anger problems should be considered as part of the routine assessment of PD, particularly within the revised framework for assessing PD being undertaken for the new (eleventh) edition of the International Classification of Diseases (ICD-11). Anger problems have their roots in early childhood. Early dysregulation at age 10 reflects a temperamental vulnerability that predicts later externalizing related traits, namely hostility, risk taking, deceitfulness and callousness [26]. It is important that children showing this pattern of dysregulation are identified early so that interventions can be undertaken to prevent it translating into adolescent and adult anger problems. The anger construct arguably needs to be refined in relation to its links with personality disorders. In particular, anger's association with positive affective states needs to be considered along with its association with negative affective states. Within each anger type, there is a need to consider the specific triggers of anger that might differentiate one personality disorder from another. Some might argue that the experience of anger is inherently unpleasant and that anger is therefore, by definition, an affect that is hedonically negative. However, a little introspection should suffice to convince the reader that anger can be experienced as pleasant. Indeed, for some individuals with a PD, the experience of excitement and exhilaration, a positive affective state, is a key motive for their violence, while others are motivated by the experience of self-gratification and social dominance at the expense of others. It remains debatable whether the “anger” experienced by such individuals is truly anger as commonly conceived. Perhaps the inverted commas should be retained to indicate its special properties in the context of this usage; for example when anger is used instrumentally to coerce another individual, as in the example of coercive anger given on p. 4.

In conclusion, it has been argued that the heterogeneous construct of anger can be de-constructed into four distinct types: thrill seeking, coercive, explosive/reactive, and vengeful/ ruminative. Each of these anger types is linked to particular types of antisocial behavior, and - in individuals with PD - to particular types of violence: violence that is motivated by a desire for excitement, for self-gratification, for self-protection, and for vengeance respectively. This theoretical framework might prove useful in future studies of antisocial behavior in individuals with personality disorders.

DISCLOSURE

The author declares no conflicts of interest.

REFERENCES

1. Averill JR. Anger and Aggression: An Essay on Emotion. New York: Springer-Verlag. 1982.

2. Plutchik R. A general psychoevolutionary theory of emotion. In: Plutchik R, & Kellerman H, editors. Emotion: Theory, Research and Experience, Volume I Theories of Emotion. New York, Academic Press. 1980; 3-33.

3. Panksepp J. Toward a general psychobiological theory of emotions. J Behav Brain Sci. 1982; 5: 407-467.

4. Howells K. Angry affect, aggression and personality disorder. McMurran M, Howard RC, editors. Personality, Personality Disorder and Violence. Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell. 2009; 191-211.

5. Howard RC. The neurobiology of affective dyscontrol: Implications for understanding ‘dangerous and severe personality disorder’. In: McMurran M, Howard RC, editors. Personality, personality disorder and violence. Chichester: Wiley. 2009; 157-174.

6. Howard RC. The quest for excitement: a missing link between personality disorder and violence? Journal of Forensic Psychiatry and Psychology. 2011; 22: 692-705.

7. American Psychiatric Association [APA]. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th edition). Arlington, VA: Author. 2013.

8. Panksepp J. Affective neuroscience of the emotional BrainMind: evolutionary perspectives and implications for understanding depression. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2010; 12: 533-545.

9. Fernandez G, Johnson SL. Anger in psychological disorders: prevalence, presentation, etiology and prognostic implications. Clin Psychol Rev. 2016; 46: 124-135.

10. Trull TJ, Schneiderer EM, Tomko RL. Axis II comorbidity. In: Widiger TA, editor. The Oxford Handbook of Personality Disorders. New York: Oxford University Press. 2012; 219-236.

11. Howard RC, Huband N, Duggan C, Mannion A. Exploring the link between personality disorder and criminality in a community sample. Journal of Personality Disorders. 2008; 22: 589-603.

12. Howard RC, Huband N, Duggan C. Adult antisocial syndrome with co-morbid borderline pathology: association with severe childhood conduct disorder. Annals of Clinical Psychiatry. 2012; 24: 127-134.

13. Howard RC, Gallagher J, Howells K, Richardson C, Tonkin M. Personality disorder, anger and institutional aggression in personality disordered offenders. Manuscript in preparation.

14. Wright AGC, Simms LJ. Stability and fluctuation of personality disorder features in daily life. J Abnorm Psychol. 2016; 125: 641-656.

15. Zanarini MC, Frankenburg FR, Vujanovic AA, Hennen J, Reich DB, Silk KR. Axis II comorbidity of borderline personality disorder: description of 6-year course and prediction to time-to-remission. Acta Psychiatrica Scandanivica. 2004; 110: 416-420.

16. McMurran M, Howard RC. Personality disorders and offending. In D. Polaschek D, Day A, Hollin CR, editors. The Wiley international handbook of correctional psychology. Chichester: Wiley Blackwell. In press. 

17. Howard RC. Personality disorders and violence: what is the link? Borderline Personality Disorder and Emotion Dysregulation. 2015; 2: 12.

18. Howard RC. Psychopathy, Impulsiveness and Violence: How are they linked? Journal of Behavior. 2017; 2(1), 1004.

19. Kotov R, Krueger RF, Watson D, Achenbach TM, Althoff RR, Bagby RM et al. The hierarchical taxonomy of psychopathology (HiTOP): a dimensional alternative to traditional nosologies. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2017; 26: 454-477.

20. Røysamb E, Kendler KS, Tambs K, Ørstavik RE, Neale MC, Aggen SH, . . . Reichborn-Kjennerud T. The joint structure of DSM-IV Axis I and Axis II disorders. J Abnorm Psychol. 2011; 120(1): 198-209.

21. Howard RC, Khalifa N. Is emotional impulsiveness (Urgency) a core feature of severe personality disorder? Personality and Individual Differences. 2016; 292: 29-32.

22. Berg JM, Latzman RD, Bliwise NG, Lilienfeld SO. Parsing the heterogeneity of impulsivity: A meta-analytic review of the behavioral implications of the UPPS for psychopathology. Psychological Assessment. 2015; 27: 1129-1146.

23. Caspi A, Houts RM, Belsky DW, Goldman-Mellor SJ, Harrington H, Israel S, et al. The p Factor: One General Psychopathology Factor in the Structure of Psychiatric Disorders? Clinical Psychological Science?: A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science. 2014; 2: 119- 137.

24. Dunne AL, Gilbert F, Daffern M. Investigating the relationship between DSM-5 Personality Disorder Domains and Facets and Aggression in an Offender Population using the Personality Inventory for the DSM-5. Journal of Personality Disorders.

25. Chanen AM, Kaess M. Developmental pathways to borderline personality disorder. Current Psychiatry Reports. 2012; 14: 45-53.

26. De Caluwe E, Decuyper M, De Clercq B. The child behavior checklist dysregulation profile predicts adolescent DSM-5 pathological personality traits 4 years later. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2013; 22: 401-411.

27. Rhee SH, Friedman NP, Corley RP, Hewitt JK, Hink LK, Johnson DP, et al. An examination of the developmental propensity model of conduct problems. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2016; 125: 550-564.

28. Moberg T, Stenbacka M, Tengström A, Jönsson EG, Nordström P, Jokinen J. Psychiatric and neurological disorders in late adolescence and risk of convictions for violent crime in men. BMC Psychiatry. 2015; 15: 299.

29. Hawes SW, Perlman SB, Byrd AL, Raine A, Loeber R, Pardini DA. Chronic anger as a precursor to adult antisocial personality features. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2016; 125: 64-67.

30. Panksepp J. Toward a general psychobiological theory of emotions. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 1982; 5: 407-467.

31. Karterud S, Pedersen G, Johansen M, Wilberg T, Davis K, Panksepp J. Primary emotional traits in patients with personality disorders. Personality and Mental Health. 2016; 10: 261-273.

32. Davis KL, Panksepp J. The brain’s emotional foundations of human personality and the Affective Neuroscience Personality scales. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Rveiews. 2011; 35: 1946-1958.

33. Kerby DS, Brand MW, John R. Anger types and the use of cigarettes and smokeless tobacco among Native American adolescents. Preventive Medicine. 2003; 37: 485-491. 

34. Linden W, Hogan BE, Rutledge T, Leung D. There is more to anger coping than "in" or "out". Emotion. 2003; 3: 12-29.

35. Chester DS, DeWall N, Derefinko KJ, Estus S, Lynam DR, Peters JR, Jiang, Y. Looking for reward in all the wrong places: dopamine receptor gene polymorphisms indirectly affect aggression through sensationseeking, Social Neuroscience. 2015; 11: 487-494.

36. Agnew RS. Strain theory and violent behavior. In: Flannery DJ, Vazsonyi AT, & Waldman ID, editors. The Cambridge handbook of violent behavior and aggression. New York: Cambridge University Press. 2007; 519-529.

37. Hutton EL,Woodworth M. Violent female youth: an examination of instrumental violence, psychopathy, and offense characteristics. Behavioral Sciences & the Law. 2014; 232: 121-134.

38. Ching H, Daffern M, Thomas SDM. Appetitive violence: A new phenomenon? Psychiatry, Psychology and Law. 2012; 19: 745–763.

39. Apter MJ. Reversal theory as a theory of the emotions. In: Apter MJ, Kerr JH, Cowles MP, editors. Progress in Reversal Theory. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers. 1988; 43-62.

40. Faccini L, Allely CS. Mass violence in individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder and Narcissistic Personality Disorder: A case analysis of Anders Breivik using the “Path to Intended and Terroristic Violence” model. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 2016; 31: 229–236.

41. Bjørnebekk G, Howard RC. Validation of a motivation-based typology of angry aggression in antisocial youth in Norway. Behavioral Sciences and the Law. 2012; 30: 167-180.

42. Bjørnebekk G, Howard RC. Sub-types of angry aggression, frequency of antisocial acts, and teachers´ perceptions of social competence, emotional and behavioural problems. Personality and Individual Differences. 2012; 53: 312-316.

43. Howard RC, Howells K, Jinks M, McMurran M. A quadripartite typology of violence (QTV): relationships with functions of aggression in violent youths. In: Needham I, Callaghan P, Palmstierna T, Nijman H, Oud N, editors. Violence in Clinical Psychiatry. The Netherlands: Kavanah. 2009; 342-345.

44. Daffern M, Howells K, Ogloff JRP. What's the point? Towards a methodology for assessing the function of psychiatric inpatient aggression. Behavior Research and Therapy. 2007; 45: 101-111.

45. Daffern M, Howells K. The functions of aggression in personality disordered offenders. Journal Of Interpersonal Violence. 2009; 24: 586- 600.

46. Sperry M. Handbook of Diagnosis and Treatment of the DSM-IV Personality Disorders. Levittown PA: Brunner/Mazel. 1995.

47. Scott LN, Wright AGC, Beeney JE, Lazarus SA, Pilkonis PA, Stepp SD. Borderline personality disorder symptoms and aggression: A withinperson process model. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2017.

48. Moore KE, Tull MT, Gratz KL. Borderline personality disorder symptoms and criminal justice system involvement: the roles of emotion-driven difficulties controlling impulsive behaviors and physical aggression. Comprehensive Psychiatry. 2017; 76: 26-35.

49. Zielinski MJ, Borders A, Giancola PR. Does hostile rumination mediate the associations between reported child abuse, parenting characteristics and borderline features in adulthood? Personality and Mental Health. 2015; 9: 288-297.

50. Caprara GV, Tisak MS, Alessandri G, Fontaine RG, Fida R, Paciello M. The contribution of moral disengagement in mediating individual tendencies toward aggression and violence. Developmental Psychology. 2014; 1: 71-85.

51. Selby EA, Joiner TE. Cascades of emotion: the emergence of borderline personality disorder from emotion and behavioural dysregulation. Review of General Psychology. 2009; 13: 219-229.

52. Selby EA, Kranzler A, Panza E, Fehling KB. Bidirectional-compounding effects of rumination and negative emotion in predicting impulsive behavior: implications for emotional cascades. Journal of Personality. 2016; 84: 139-153.

Received : 27 Jul 2017
Accepted : 20 Oct 2017
Published : 01 Dec 2017
Journals
Annals of Otolaryngology and Rhinology
ISSN : 2379-948X
Launched : 2014
JSM Schizophrenia
Launched : 2016
Journal of Nausea
Launched : 2020
JSM Internal Medicine
Launched : 2016
JSM Hepatitis
Launched : 2016
JSM Oro Facial Surgeries
ISSN : 2578-3211
Launched : 2016
Journal of Human Nutrition and Food Science
ISSN : 2333-6706
Launched : 2013
JSM Regenerative Medicine and Bioengineering
ISSN : 2379-0490
Launched : 2013
JSM Spine
ISSN : 2578-3181
Launched : 2016
Archives of Palliative Care
ISSN : 2573-1165
Launched : 2016
JSM Nutritional Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3203
Launched : 2017
Annals of Neurodegenerative Disorders
ISSN : 2476-2032
Launched : 2016
Journal of Fever
ISSN : 2641-7782
Launched : 2017
JSM Bone Marrow Research
ISSN : 2578-3351
Launched : 2016
JSM Mathematics and Statistics
ISSN : 2578-3173
Launched : 2014
Journal of Autoimmunity and Research
ISSN : 2573-1173
Launched : 2014
JSM Arthritis
ISSN : 2475-9155
Launched : 2016
JSM Head and Neck Cancer-Cases and Reviews
ISSN : 2573-1610
Launched : 2016
JSM General Surgery Cases and Images
ISSN : 2573-1564
Launched : 2016
JSM Anatomy and Physiology
ISSN : 2573-1262
Launched : 2016
JSM Dental Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1548
Launched : 2016
Annals of Emergency Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1017
Launched : 2016
Annals of Mens Health and Wellness
ISSN : 2641-7707
Launched : 2017
Journal of Preventive Medicine and Health Care
ISSN : 2576-0084
Launched : 2018
Journal of Chronic Diseases and Management
ISSN : 2573-1300
Launched : 2016
Annals of Vaccines and Immunization
ISSN : 2378-9379
Launched : 2014
JSM Heart Surgery Cases and Images
ISSN : 2578-3157
Launched : 2016
Annals of Reproductive Medicine and Treatment
ISSN : 2573-1092
Launched : 2016
JSM Brain Science
ISSN : 2573-1289
Launched : 2016
JSM Biomarkers
ISSN : 2578-3815
Launched : 2014
JSM Biology
ISSN : 2475-9392
Launched : 2016
Archives of Stem Cell and Research
ISSN : 2578-3580
Launched : 2014
Annals of Clinical and Medical Microbiology
ISSN : 2578-3629
Launched : 2014
JSM Pediatric Surgery
ISSN : 2578-3149
Launched : 2017
Journal of Memory Disorder and Rehabilitation
ISSN : 2578-319X
Launched : 2016
JSM Tropical Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2578-3165
Launched : 2016
JSM Head and Face Medicine
ISSN : 2578-3793
Launched : 2016
JSM Cardiothoracic Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1297
Launched : 2016
JSM Bone and Joint Diseases
ISSN : 2578-3351
Launched : 2017
JSM Bioavailability and Bioequivalence
ISSN : 2641-7812
Launched : 2017
JSM Atherosclerosis
ISSN : 2573-1270
Launched : 2016
Journal of Genitourinary Disorders
ISSN : 2641-7790
Launched : 2017
Journal of Fractures and Sprains
ISSN : 2578-3831
Launched : 2016
Journal of Autism and Epilepsy
ISSN : 2641-7774
Launched : 2016
Annals of Marine Biology and Research
ISSN : 2573-105X
Launched : 2014
JSM Health Education & Primary Health Care
ISSN : 2578-3777
Launched : 2016
JSM Communication Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3807
Launched : 2016
Annals of Musculoskeletal Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3599
Launched : 2016
Annals of Virology and Research
ISSN : 2573-1122
Launched : 2014
JSM Renal Medicine
ISSN : 2573-1637
Launched : 2016
Journal of Muscle Health
ISSN : 2578-3823
Launched : 2016
JSM Genetics and Genomics
ISSN : 2334-1823
Launched : 2013
JSM Anxiety and Depression
ISSN : 2475-9139
Launched : 2016
Clinical Journal of Heart Diseases
ISSN : 2641-7766
Launched : 2016
Annals of Medicinal Chemistry and Research
ISSN : 2378-9336
Launched : 2014
JSM Pain and Management
ISSN : 2578-3378
Launched : 2016
JSM Women's Health
ISSN : 2578-3696
Launched : 2016
Clinical Research in HIV or AIDS
ISSN : 2374-0094
Launched : 2013
Journal of Endocrinology, Diabetes and Obesity
ISSN : 2333-6692
Launched : 2013
Journal of Substance Abuse and Alcoholism
ISSN : 2373-9363
Launched : 2013
JSM Neurosurgery and Spine
ISSN : 2373-9479
Launched : 2013
Journal of Liver and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2379-0830
Launched : 2014
Journal of Drug Design and Research
ISSN : 2379-089X
Launched : 2014
JSM Clinical Oncology and Research
ISSN : 2373-938X
Launched : 2013
JSM Bioinformatics, Genomics and Proteomics
ISSN : 2576-1102
Launched : 2014
JSM Chemistry
ISSN : 2334-1831
Launched : 2013
Journal of Trauma and Care
ISSN : 2573-1246
Launched : 2014
JSM Surgical Oncology and Research
ISSN : 2578-3688
Launched : 2016
Annals of Food Processing and Preservation
ISSN : 2573-1033
Launched : 2016
Journal of Radiology and Radiation Therapy
ISSN : 2333-7095
Launched : 2013
JSM Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation
ISSN : 2578-3572
Launched : 2016
Annals of Clinical Pathology
ISSN : 2373-9282
Launched : 2013
Annals of Cardiovascular Diseases
ISSN : 2641-7731
Launched : 2016
Annals of Clinical and Experimental Metabolism
ISSN : 2572-2492
Launched : 2016
Clinical Research in Infectious Diseases
ISSN : 2379-0636
Launched : 2013
JSM Microbiology
ISSN : 2333-6455
Launched : 2013
Journal of Urology and Research
ISSN : 2379-951X
Launched : 2014
Journal of Family Medicine and Community Health
ISSN : 2379-0547
Launched : 2013
Annals of Pregnancy and Care
ISSN : 2578-336X
Launched : 2017
JSM Cell and Developmental Biology
ISSN : 2379-061X
Launched : 2013
Annals of Aquaculture and Research
ISSN : 2379-0881
Launched : 2014
Clinical Research in Pulmonology
ISSN : 2333-6625
Launched : 2013
Journal of Immunology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2333-6714
Launched : 2013
Annals of Forensic Research and Analysis
ISSN : 2378-9476
Launched : 2014
JSM Biochemistry and Molecular Biology
ISSN : 2333-7109
Launched : 2013
Annals of Breast Cancer Research
ISSN : 2641-7685
Launched : 2016
Annals of Gerontology and Geriatric Research
ISSN : 2378-9409
Launched : 2014
Journal of Sleep Medicine and Disorders
ISSN : 2379-0822
Launched : 2014
JSM Burns and Trauma
ISSN : 2475-9406
Launched : 2016
Chemical Engineering and Process Techniques
ISSN : 2333-6633
Launched : 2013
Annals of Clinical Cytology and Pathology
ISSN : 2475-9430
Launched : 2014
JSM Allergy and Asthma
ISSN : 2573-1254
Launched : 2016
Journal of Neurological Disorders and Stroke
ISSN : 2334-2307
Launched : 2013
Annals of Sports Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2379-0571
Launched : 2014
JSM Sexual Medicine
ISSN : 2578-3718
Launched : 2016
Annals of Vascular Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2378-9344
Launched : 2014
JSM Biotechnology and Biomedical Engineering
ISSN : 2333-7117
Launched : 2013
Journal of Hematology and Transfusion
ISSN : 2333-6684
Launched : 2013
JSM Environmental Science and Ecology
ISSN : 2333-7141
Launched : 2013
Journal of Cardiology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2333-6676
Launched : 2013
JSM Nanotechnology and Nanomedicine
ISSN : 2334-1815
Launched : 2013
Journal of Ear, Nose and Throat Disorders
ISSN : 2475-9473
Launched : 2016
JSM Ophthalmology
ISSN : 2333-6447
Launched : 2013
Journal of Pharmacology and Clinical Toxicology
ISSN : 2333-7079
Launched : 2013
Annals of Psychiatry and Mental Health
ISSN : 2374-0124
Launched : 2013
Medical Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology
ISSN : 2333-6439
Launched : 2013
Annals of Pediatrics and Child Health
ISSN : 2373-9312
Launched : 2013
JSM Clinical Pharmaceutics
ISSN : 2379-9498
Launched : 2014
JSM Foot and Ankle
ISSN : 2475-9112
Launched : 2016
JSM Alzheimer's Disease and Related Dementia
ISSN : 2378-9565
Launched : 2014
Journal of Addiction Medicine and Therapy
ISSN : 2333-665X
Launched : 2013
Journal of Veterinary Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2378-931X
Launched : 2013
Annals of Public Health and Research
ISSN : 2378-9328
Launched : 2014
Annals of Orthopedics and Rheumatology
ISSN : 2373-9290
Launched : 2013
Journal of Clinical Nephrology and Research
ISSN : 2379-0652
Launched : 2014
Annals of Community Medicine and Practice
ISSN : 2475-9465
Launched : 2014
Annals of Biometrics and Biostatistics
ISSN : 2374-0116
Launched : 2013
JSM Clinical Case Reports
ISSN : 2373-9819
Launched : 2013
Journal of Cancer Biology and Research
ISSN : 2373-9436
Launched : 2013
Journal of Surgery and Transplantation Science
ISSN : 2379-0911
Launched : 2013
Journal of Dermatology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2373-9371
Launched : 2013
JSM Gastroenterology and Hepatology
ISSN : 2373-9487
Launched : 2013
Annals of Nursing and Practice
ISSN : 2379-9501
Launched : 2014
JSM Dentistry
ISSN : 2333-7133
Launched : 2013
Author Information X