Loading

Journal of Preventive Medicine and Health Care

Assessment of Mental Imagery of Movement in Schizophrenia

Research Article | Open Access | Volume 5 | Issue 1

  • 1. Department of Obstetrics, University of Western Macedonia, Greece
  • 2. Cognitive Psychologists, Greece
  • 3. Department Psychiatric Clinic, Neuropsychologist, AHEPA Hospital Thessaloniki, Greece
  • 4. Director of the CAPS laboratory, University of Burgundy, Dijon, France
+ Show More - Show Less
Corresponding Authors
Theofilidis Antonis*, Department of Obstetrics, University of Western Macedonia, Greece; Department Psychiatric Clinic, Neuropsychologist, AHEPA Hospital Thessaloniki, Greece
Abstract

Schizophrenia patients often present with neurological signs such as left/right confusion, impaired coordination of movements, and motor abnormalities may occur secondary to antipsychotic medications. Neuropsychological tests identify various dysfunctions, such as difficulty in focusing attention, difficulty in abstract thinking or difficulty in changing the response frame, while Neurophysiological tests show slowing down of reaction time, problems with eye tracking, etc. There are no studies in the international literature dealing with the investigation of mental imagery of movement in Greek patients with schizophrenia. Also, the position that patients with schizophrenia – regardless of their symptom profile, age, sex, and chronicity of the disease – have a permanent difficulty in creating and manipulating an internal model of their movement prediction, has not been substantiated enough to date.

Aim: In the present study we examine the function of visual-motor coordination and the time to achieve the goal-directed movement of the dominant hand of patients with schizophrenia

Method: The sample we used in our research consisted of patients with diagnosed schizophrenia and healthy individuals. Schizophrenia patients belonged to the experimental group and healthy subjects to the control group. The number of examined patients was 39 and the healthy 51. the assessment of the movement and its mental representation was carried out with the real and mental movement of the dominant hand between two square targets located at a distance of 20 cm, on white paper of A4 size.

Conclusions: Visuomotor coordination of a specific hand movement differs between patients with schizophrenia and the normal population. In particular, the means of the goaldirected hand movement achievement time in the actual execution condition and in the mental execution condition of the patients were significantly higher than the corresponding times of the standard population

Keywords

Schizophrenia, Mental imagery of movement

Citation

Antonis T, George S, Maria S, Margarita H, Konstantinos T, et al. (2023) Assessment of Mental Imagery of Movement in Schizophrenia. J Prev Med Healthc 5(1): 1028.

INTRODUCTION

Neuropsychological deficits are recognized as primary symptoms of psychotic disorders, since they are detected even in the early stages, with some of them showing a more significant decline (e.g. executive functions, memory, attention) and others a smaller but significant one (e.g. language, movement perception, psychomotor). A wealth of neuropsychological research over the past decade has contributed to elucidating the nature and significance of cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia. Today we know that patients with schizophrenia show lower performance in all known neuropsychological tests compared to healthy controls.

Schizophrenic deficits in psychomotor speed

We know that schizophrenia is governed by deficits in psychomotor speed [1,2]. Psychomotor speed refers to the time to process a stimulus, prepare the response to that stimulus, and execute the response 1]. It has been shown that patients with schizophrenia are disadvantaged compared to healthy individuals in tests of psychomotor speed [1,2]. However, this difference does not always reach the significance level [3]. Of interest is the result of the research by Riley and his colleagues (2000) that deficits in psychomotor speed in schizophrenia appear as early as the first episode, reinforcing the view that this area is greatly damaged by the presence of psychotic symptoms [4]. Sweeney et al.’s (1991) study found improvement in psychomotor speed in both first-episode schizophrenia patients and patients with a previous history of psychosis [5], which is consistent with the view that neurocognitive deficits in patients with schizophrenia remain relatively stable across the lifespan [1,4].

The depiction of movement

Movement imagery is a mental process in which a specific movement is internally simulated without actually performing the movement. According to simulation theory, mental movements are essentially movements that do not take place [6,7]. Much research has highlighted the relationship between real movement and its corresponding mental representation as well as common brain areas that are activated during planning and execution in both cases, while both mental and real movement follow the same rules (e.g. speed-accuracy, speed-curvature relationship) and maintain the same temporal structure [6,8-11]. In addition, mental training improves motor performance [12-14] and enhances muscle strength [15-17]. Neuroimaging studies have revealed a common activation of specific brain regions during both motor and mental execution of a movement (parietal and prefrontal cortex, primary cortex, basal ganglia and cerebellum) [7,18-21].

Research using movement imagery tests is a useful and sensitive method of investigating the unconscious process of representation [6] (Figure 1). Movement imagery has been widely used to investigate the movement system in both healthy and clinical populations. The advantage of investigating kinesthetic imaging is that the internal processes of real movement that involve planning and planning can be studied. This element is very important for the study of the production and execution of the movement and the related deficits shown by patients of various categories such as patients with psychiatric or neurological syndromes. In these patient cases the ability or inability to produce or perform a movement can indicate whether the relevant brain function has begun to be affected, to what extent it is deficient, and the degree of progressive impairment.

Imagery tests.

Figure 1 Imagery tests.
Deficits in mental imagery of movement have been assessed in Parkinson’s disease [22-24], prefrontal cortex syndrome [25], motor - sensory cortex [26,27], and in multiple sclerosis [28,29]. Investigating the mental imagery of movement also helps psychomotor retardation (Psychomotor retardation-PMR-) which is a central factor in the assessment of clinical and therapeutic effects and can seriously affect the psychosocial functioning of patients. Psychomotor rehabilitation modifies all of the person’s actions, including mobility, mental activity, and speech [30].

METHODOLOGY

Research case

Based on the preceding literature review, it became clear that patients with schizophrenia present many cognitive deficits, the most important of which are related to executive functions, memory, and attention. The aim of our own research was to investigate the target-directed movement ability of the dominant hand of schizophrenics and to compare this ability with healthy individuals in order to clarify the possible difference between the two groups. Our research question has not been answered in any research in the Greek area and we hope that it will be another important element in being able to clarify with the greatest possible precision the deficits in the cognitive functions of schizophrenic patients.

Sample

The sample we used in our research consisted of patients with diagnosed schizophrenia and healthy individuals. Schizophrenia patients belonged to the experimental group and healthy subjects to the control group. The number of patients examined was 39 and the healthy 51. The age range of the patients was from 28 to 64 years with a mean age of 45.3 years. Nine were men and six were women. Seven had completed secondary education, five tertiary and three primaries. The group of healthy subjects was matched based on all the above elements with the group of schizophrenics. All participants had normal vision and were right-handed. The patients had previously been treated at the 3rd University Psychiatric Clinic of the AHEPA Hospital in Thessaloniki and participated in the study during their scheduled visit to the clinic’s outpatient clinics. Exclusion criteria from the study were: depression, neurological disease, severe organic disease, substance use. Also, those patients receiving medication (Haloperidol, Risperidone, Biperidone) which can affect movement were excluded. The patients in the sample were receiving second generation antipsychotics (Second Generation Antipsychotics SGAs – Clozapine up to 900mg/24h, Olanzapine up to 20mg/24h, Aripiprazole up to 30mg/24h, Cariprazine up to 6mg/24h, Amisoulpride, Lourasidone, Ziprasidone). Control group participants were free from any neurological, cognitive and organic impairment and were randomly selected from the community. All participants gave written consent to participate in the study. The research protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Medicine of the A.U.Th. and conducted in accordance with the principle set out in the Declaration of Helsinki.

Object of the study - Research questions

In the present study, the function of visual-motor coordination and the time to achieve the goal-directed movement of the dominant hand of patients with schizophrenia was investigated. The questions to be answered were:

  • What differences are there in the time to achieve the specific goal-directed hand movement between patients and the typical population?
  • Are there differences in the time to achieve the specific goal-directed hand movement in the actual execution condition and in the mental execution condition of the movement between patients and standard population?
  • Does visuomotor coordination and the time to achieve the specific hand movement differ in patients with schizophrenia according to their age, gender and educational level?

The hypotheses of the research

  • Hypothesis 1: the visuomotor coordination of the specific hand movement between patients and the typical population will show large discrepancies in reaching speed and target accuracy.
  • Hypothesis 2: Motor imagery will be deficient in patients with schizophrenia. Both the simplification of the movement task and the repetitions (learning) of the movement will not affect the time of achievement and its accuracy.

Hypothesis 3: the time to achieve the specific goal-directed movement of the dominant hand of patients with schizophrenia will differ according to real vs mental movement. 

Data collection method - Experimental protocol

Data collection took place in a quiet office. In order to limit the influence of circadian rhythms on motor and mental performance [32,33] all experiments were performed in the morning (between 9 and 11 AM). Participants were seated comfortably in an adjustable chair in front of a table whose edge was aligned with their chest at the level of the diaphragm. In the middle of the table, the pieces of paper (A4 format) were placed at a distance of 20 cm from the chest of the participants. Three targets (black squares) and a starting point (abference) were printed on each sheet. We used four different target sizes (0.5cm, 1cm, 1.5cm, 2cm) and the distances between the targets were (15cm) to model the test according to Fitts’ law [34].

Participants had to perform (actual movement) (Table 1) or imagine themselves performing the movement (mental movement) between the targets as accurately and as quickly as possible (i.e., we adapted Fitts’ law motor paradigm, see also [35], while the presentation of the targets was randomized to exclude the learning process. After the instructions were briefed and explained, participants were free to initiate the real or mental movements when they felt ready. /s. Each real or mental movement was performed eight times between targets for each trial (0.5 cm, 1 cm, 1.5 cm, 2 cm). For mental movements, participants were asked to place their pencil over the original target (starting point), hold it still throughout the trial, and feel that they are performing the task (motor) as they would in reality. Of a movement at the mental level is a necessary condition for the motor system to be involved [36,37]. For each actual test, we measured the reaching time as well as the accuracy of the movements.

Table 1: Actual movements and mental movements.

 

Patients

Normal

 

Actual movement – target 0.5X0.5 cm

Μ=34,97, SD=9,797

M=18,57, SD=6,152

t=60,215 p=.000

Actual movement – target 1X1 cm

Μ=30,21, SD=8,408

M=15,96, SD=4,927

t=57,563 p=.000

Actual movement – target 1.5X1.5 cm

Μ=27,46, SD=6,95

M=14,57, SD=4,041

t=57,25 p=.000

Actual movement – target 2X2 cm

Μ=25,67, SD=7,432

M=13,53, SD=3,733

t=52,59 p=.000

Μental movement – target 0.5X0.5 cm

Μ=25,28, SD=7,96

M=15,39, SD=4,133

t=53,55 p=.000

Μental movement – target 1X1 cm

Μ=24,62, SD=7,738

M=14,90, SD=4,016

t=53,54 p=.000

Μental movement – target 1.5X1.5 cm

Μ=23,31, SD=6,795

M=14,04, SD=3,873

t=56,573 p=.000

Μental movement – target 2X2 cm

Μ=23,31, SD=7,179

M=13,55, SD=3,786

t=54,025 p=.000

Participants were told that if they missed more than two targets during a trial, it would be canceled (invalid trial) and repeated at the end of the session. No tests had to be repeated. Each test was performed on separate sheets. The main experiment was preceded by a series of practice trials, which allowed participants to familiarize themselves with the task. The targets used in practice were of different sizes (3 cm and 2.5 cm) than those used in the experiment. After this practice phase, all participants verbally reported that they were able to generate motor imagery after having practiced 9–10 times. During the experiment, all participants performed eight real and eight mental trials for each different target dimension (64 trials per participant) in random order. When participants completed eight consecutive trials, they rested for ∼1 min to avoid physical and mental fatigue. After achieving the experimental protocol, none of the participants reported mental or physical fatigue and any difficulty in internally simulating the movements.

RESULTS

Regarding gender, the sample consists of 42 men (46.7%) and 48 women (53.3%). Regarding the disease, 51 normal (56.7%) and 39 patients with schizophrenia (43.3%) participated. Of all the participants in the research, 2 people had completed primary education (2.2%), 12 people (13.3%) high school, 23 people high school (25.6%) 37 people (41.1%) the university while 16 people had also completed postgraduate education (17.8%). 27 people (30%) were aged 19 to 29, 19 people (21%) were aged 30 to 39, 12 people (13%) were aged 40 to 49, 23 people (26%) were aged 50 to 59 and 9 people (10%) belonged to the age group of 60 to seventy years.

Control of performance averages

We calculated the averages of the time to achieve the specific goal-directed real and mental movement to targets of different dimensions (square targets with dimension: 0.5X0.5 cm, 1X1 cm, 1.5X1.5 cm, and 2X2 cm). Testing the means with the t-test for independent samples showed that reaching time was significantly associated with schizophrenia in all cases (real and mental movement and different goal dimensions). Patients with schizophrenia perform this movement in a longer period of time than the normal population.

CONCLUSIONS

Τhe execution time of both real and mental movement in all subtests (target size) is longer for patients with schizophrenia compared to the corresponding times of normal subjects.

As the dimension of the target increases, the time to achieve the movement decreases for both conditions (real and mental movement) and for both groups (normal population - patients with schizophrenia).

Schizophrenia patients show larger differences between the mean execution time between the actual and mental execution of the movement than the corresponding differences of the normal population.

Τhe difference in the averages of the time to achieve the movement is smaller between the two groups (normal - schizophrenic) for the condition of mental execution of the specific movement.

REFERENCES
  1. Ekerholm M, Waltersson SF, Fagerberg T, Söderman E, Terenius L, Agartz I, et al. Neurocognitive function in long-term treated schizophrenia: a five-year follow-up study. Psychiatry Res. 2012; 200: 144-152.
  2. Schretlen DJ, Cascella NG, Meyer SM, Kingery LR, Testa SM, Munro CA, et al. Neuropsychological functioning in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2007; 62: 179-186.
  3. Bozikas VP, Kosmidis MH, Kiosseoglou G, Karavatos A. Neuropsychological profile of cognitively impaired patients with schizophrenia. Compr Psychiatry. 2007; 47: 136-143.
  4. Townsend LA, Norman RM. Course of cognitive functioning in first episode schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Expert Rev Neurother. 2004; 4: 61-68.
  5. Rund BR. A review of longitudinal studies of cognitive function in schizophrenia patients. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 1998; 24: 425-435.
  6. Decety J, Jeannerod M. Mentally simulated movements in virtual

    reality: does Fitts’s law hold in motor imagery?. Behav Brain Res. 1995; 72: 127-134.

  7. Jeannerod M. Neural simulation of action: a unifying mechanism for motor cognition. Neuroimage. 2001; 14: S103-109.
  8. Maruff P, Wilson PH, De Fazio J, Cerritelli B, Hedt A, Currie J. Asymmetries between dominant and non-dominant hands in real and imagined motor task performance. Neuropsychologia. 1999; 37: 379-384.
  9. Bakker M, De Lange FP, Stevens JA, Toni I, Bloem BR. Motor imagery of gait: a quantitative approach. Exp Brain Res. 2007; 179: 497-504.
  10. Gueugneau N, Crognier L, Papaxanthis C. The influence of eye movements on the temporal features of executed and imagined arm movements. Brain Res. 2008; 1187: 95-102.
  11. Papaxanthis C, Paizis C, White O, Pozzo T, Stucchi N. The Relation between Geometry and Time in Mental Actions. PloS One. 2012; 7: e51191.
  12. Yaguez L, Nagel D, Hoffman H, Canavan AG, Wist E, Homberg V. A mental route to motor learning: improving trajectorial kinematics through imagery training. Behav Brain Res. 1998; 90: 95-106.
  13. Gentili R, Papaxanthis C, Pozzo T. Improvement and generalization of arm motor performance through motor imagery practice. Neuroscience. 2006; 137: 761-772.
  14. Avanzino L, Giannini A, Tacchino A, Pelosin E, Ruggeri P, Bove M. Motor imagery influences the execution of repetitive finger opposition movements. Neuroscience letters. 2009; 466: 11-15.
  15. Yue G, Cole KJ. Strength increases from the motor program: comparison of training with maximal voluntary and imagined muscle contractions. J Neurophysiol. 1992; 67: 1114-1123.
  16. Zijdewind I, Toering ST, Bessem B, Laan OVD, Diercks RL. Effects of imagery motor training on torque production of ankle plantar flexor muscles. Muscle Nerve. 2003; 28: 168-173.
  17. Ranganathan VK, Siemionow V, Liu JZ, Sahgal V, Yue GH. From mental power to muscle power--gaining strength by using the mind. Neuropsychologia. 2004; 42: 944-956.
  18. Guillot A, Collet C. Contribution from neurophysiological and psychological methods to the study of motor imagery. Brain Res Rev. 2005; 50: 387-397.
  19. Szameitat AJ, Shen S, Sterr A. Motor imagery of complex everyday movements. An fMRI study. Neuroimage. 2007; 34: 702-713.
  20. Munzert J, Lorey B, Zentgraf K. Cognitive motor processes: the role of motor imagery in the study of motor representations. Brain Res Rev. 2009; 60: 306-326.
  21. Hetu S, Gregoire M, Saimpont A, Coll MP, Eugene F, Michon PE. The neural network of motor imagery: an ALE meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2013; 37: 930-949.
  22. Dominey P, Decety J, Broussolle E, Chazot G, Jeannerod M. Motor imagery of a lateralized sequential task is asymmetrically slowed inhemi-Parkinson’s patients. Neuropsychologia. 1995; 33: 727-741.
  23. Helmich RC, De Lange FP, Bloem BR, Toni I. Cerebral compensation during motor imagery in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia. 2007; 45: 2201-2215.
  24. Heremans E, Feys P, Nieuwboer A, Vercruysse S, Vandenberghe W, Sharma N, et al. Motor imagery ability in patients with early- and mid- stage Parkinson disease. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2011; 25: 168-177.
  25. Kagerer FA, Bracha V, Wunderlich DA, Stelmach GE, Bloedel JR. Ataxia reflected in the simulated movements of patients with cerebellar lesions. Exp Brain Res. 1998; 121: 125-134.
  26. Sirigu A, Duhamel JR, Cohen L, Pillon B, Dubois B, Agid Y. The mental representation of hand movements after parietal cortex damage. Science. 1996; 273: 1564-1568.
  27. Danckert J, Ferber S, Doherty T, Steinmetz H, Nicolle D, Goodale MA. Selective, non-lateralized impairment of motor imagery following right parietal damage. Neurocase. 2004; 8: 194-204.
  28. Heremans E, D’hooge AM, De Bondt S, Helsen W, Feys P. The relation between cognitive and motor dysfunction and motor imagery ability in patients with multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler. 2012; 18: 1303-1309.
  29. Tacchino A, Bove M, Pedulla L, Battaglia MA, Papaxanthis C, BrichettoG. Imagined actions in multiple sclerosis patients: evidence of decline in motor cognitive prediction. Exp Brain Res. 2013; 229: 561-570.
  30. Widlocher DJ. Psychomotor retardation: clinical, theoretical, and psychometric aspects. Psychiatr Clin North Am. 1983; 6: 27-40.
  31. Delerue C, Boucart M. Visual exploration and action processing in schizophrenia. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2013; 18: 153–167.
  32. Gueugneau N, Mauvieux B, Papaxanthis C. Circadian modulation of mentally simulated motor actions: implications for the potential use of motor imagery in rehabilitation. Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair. 2009; 23: 237-245.
  33. Gueugneau N, Bove M, Avanzino L, Jacquin A, Pozzo T, PapaxanthisC. Interhemispheric inhibition during mental actions of different complexity. PLoS One. 2013; 8: e56973.
  34. Fitts PM. The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement 1954. J Exp Psychol Gen. 1992; 121: 262-269.
  35. Maruff P, Wilson PH, De Fazio J, Cerritelli B, Hedt A, Currie J. Asymmetries between dominant and non-dominant hands in real and imagined motor task performance. Neuropsychologia. 1999; 37: 379-384.
  36. Stinear CM, Byblow WD, Steyvers M, Levin O, Swinnen SP. Kinesthetic, but not visual, motor imagery modulates corticomotor excitability. Exp Brain Res. 2006; 168: 157-164.
  37. McIntosh AM, Job DE, Moorhead TWJ, Harrison LK, Lawrie SM, Johnstone EC. White matter density in patients with schizophrenia, bipolar disorder and their unaffected relatives. Biol Psychiatry. 2005; 58: 254-257.

Antonis T, George S, Maria S, Margarita H, Konstantinos T, et al. (2023) Assessment of Mental Imagery of Movement in Schizophrenia. J Prev
Med Healthc 5(1): 1028.

Received : 22 Mar 2023
Accepted : 14 Apr 2023
Published : 15 Apr 2023
Journals
Annals of Otolaryngology and Rhinology
ISSN : 2379-948X
Launched : 2014
JSM Schizophrenia
Launched : 2016
Journal of Nausea
Launched : 2020
JSM Internal Medicine
Launched : 2016
JSM Hepatitis
Launched : 2016
JSM Oro Facial Surgeries
ISSN : 2578-3211
Launched : 2016
Journal of Human Nutrition and Food Science
ISSN : 2333-6706
Launched : 2013
JSM Regenerative Medicine and Bioengineering
ISSN : 2379-0490
Launched : 2013
JSM Spine
ISSN : 2578-3181
Launched : 2016
Archives of Palliative Care
ISSN : 2573-1165
Launched : 2016
JSM Nutritional Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3203
Launched : 2017
Annals of Neurodegenerative Disorders
ISSN : 2476-2032
Launched : 2016
Journal of Fever
ISSN : 2641-7782
Launched : 2017
JSM Bone Marrow Research
ISSN : 2578-3351
Launched : 2016
JSM Mathematics and Statistics
ISSN : 2578-3173
Launched : 2014
Journal of Autoimmunity and Research
ISSN : 2573-1173
Launched : 2014
JSM Arthritis
ISSN : 2475-9155
Launched : 2016
JSM Head and Neck Cancer-Cases and Reviews
ISSN : 2573-1610
Launched : 2016
JSM General Surgery Cases and Images
ISSN : 2573-1564
Launched : 2016
JSM Anatomy and Physiology
ISSN : 2573-1262
Launched : 2016
JSM Dental Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1548
Launched : 2016
Annals of Emergency Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1017
Launched : 2016
Annals of Mens Health and Wellness
ISSN : 2641-7707
Launched : 2017
Journal of Chronic Diseases and Management
ISSN : 2573-1300
Launched : 2016
Annals of Vaccines and Immunization
ISSN : 2378-9379
Launched : 2014
JSM Heart Surgery Cases and Images
ISSN : 2578-3157
Launched : 2016
Annals of Reproductive Medicine and Treatment
ISSN : 2573-1092
Launched : 2016
JSM Brain Science
ISSN : 2573-1289
Launched : 2016
JSM Biomarkers
ISSN : 2578-3815
Launched : 2014
JSM Biology
ISSN : 2475-9392
Launched : 2016
Archives of Stem Cell and Research
ISSN : 2578-3580
Launched : 2014
Annals of Clinical and Medical Microbiology
ISSN : 2578-3629
Launched : 2014
JSM Pediatric Surgery
ISSN : 2578-3149
Launched : 2017
Journal of Memory Disorder and Rehabilitation
ISSN : 2578-319X
Launched : 2016
JSM Tropical Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2578-3165
Launched : 2016
JSM Head and Face Medicine
ISSN : 2578-3793
Launched : 2016
JSM Cardiothoracic Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1297
Launched : 2016
JSM Bone and Joint Diseases
ISSN : 2578-3351
Launched : 2017
JSM Bioavailability and Bioequivalence
ISSN : 2641-7812
Launched : 2017
JSM Atherosclerosis
ISSN : 2573-1270
Launched : 2016
Journal of Genitourinary Disorders
ISSN : 2641-7790
Launched : 2017
Journal of Fractures and Sprains
ISSN : 2578-3831
Launched : 2016
Journal of Autism and Epilepsy
ISSN : 2641-7774
Launched : 2016
Annals of Marine Biology and Research
ISSN : 2573-105X
Launched : 2014
JSM Health Education & Primary Health Care
ISSN : 2578-3777
Launched : 2016
JSM Communication Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3807
Launched : 2016
Annals of Musculoskeletal Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3599
Launched : 2016
Annals of Virology and Research
ISSN : 2573-1122
Launched : 2014
JSM Renal Medicine
ISSN : 2573-1637
Launched : 2016
Journal of Muscle Health
ISSN : 2578-3823
Launched : 2016
JSM Genetics and Genomics
ISSN : 2334-1823
Launched : 2013
JSM Anxiety and Depression
ISSN : 2475-9139
Launched : 2016
Clinical Journal of Heart Diseases
ISSN : 2641-7766
Launched : 2016
Annals of Medicinal Chemistry and Research
ISSN : 2378-9336
Launched : 2014
JSM Pain and Management
ISSN : 2578-3378
Launched : 2016
JSM Women's Health
ISSN : 2578-3696
Launched : 2016
Clinical Research in HIV or AIDS
ISSN : 2374-0094
Launched : 2013
Journal of Endocrinology, Diabetes and Obesity
ISSN : 2333-6692
Launched : 2013
Journal of Substance Abuse and Alcoholism
ISSN : 2373-9363
Launched : 2013
JSM Neurosurgery and Spine
ISSN : 2373-9479
Launched : 2013
Journal of Liver and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2379-0830
Launched : 2014
Journal of Drug Design and Research
ISSN : 2379-089X
Launched : 2014
JSM Clinical Oncology and Research
ISSN : 2373-938X
Launched : 2013
JSM Bioinformatics, Genomics and Proteomics
ISSN : 2576-1102
Launched : 2014
JSM Chemistry
ISSN : 2334-1831
Launched : 2013
Journal of Trauma and Care
ISSN : 2573-1246
Launched : 2014
JSM Surgical Oncology and Research
ISSN : 2578-3688
Launched : 2016
Annals of Food Processing and Preservation
ISSN : 2573-1033
Launched : 2016
Journal of Radiology and Radiation Therapy
ISSN : 2333-7095
Launched : 2013
JSM Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation
ISSN : 2578-3572
Launched : 2016
Annals of Clinical Pathology
ISSN : 2373-9282
Launched : 2013
Annals of Cardiovascular Diseases
ISSN : 2641-7731
Launched : 2016
Journal of Behavior
ISSN : 2576-0076
Launched : 2016
Annals of Clinical and Experimental Metabolism
ISSN : 2572-2492
Launched : 2016
Clinical Research in Infectious Diseases
ISSN : 2379-0636
Launched : 2013
JSM Microbiology
ISSN : 2333-6455
Launched : 2013
Journal of Urology and Research
ISSN : 2379-951X
Launched : 2014
Journal of Family Medicine and Community Health
ISSN : 2379-0547
Launched : 2013
Annals of Pregnancy and Care
ISSN : 2578-336X
Launched : 2017
JSM Cell and Developmental Biology
ISSN : 2379-061X
Launched : 2013
Annals of Aquaculture and Research
ISSN : 2379-0881
Launched : 2014
Clinical Research in Pulmonology
ISSN : 2333-6625
Launched : 2013
Journal of Immunology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2333-6714
Launched : 2013
Annals of Forensic Research and Analysis
ISSN : 2378-9476
Launched : 2014
JSM Biochemistry and Molecular Biology
ISSN : 2333-7109
Launched : 2013
Annals of Breast Cancer Research
ISSN : 2641-7685
Launched : 2016
Annals of Gerontology and Geriatric Research
ISSN : 2378-9409
Launched : 2014
Journal of Sleep Medicine and Disorders
ISSN : 2379-0822
Launched : 2014
JSM Burns and Trauma
ISSN : 2475-9406
Launched : 2016
Chemical Engineering and Process Techniques
ISSN : 2333-6633
Launched : 2013
Annals of Clinical Cytology and Pathology
ISSN : 2475-9430
Launched : 2014
JSM Allergy and Asthma
ISSN : 2573-1254
Launched : 2016
Journal of Neurological Disorders and Stroke
ISSN : 2334-2307
Launched : 2013
Annals of Sports Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2379-0571
Launched : 2014
JSM Sexual Medicine
ISSN : 2578-3718
Launched : 2016
Annals of Vascular Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2378-9344
Launched : 2014
JSM Biotechnology and Biomedical Engineering
ISSN : 2333-7117
Launched : 2013
Journal of Hematology and Transfusion
ISSN : 2333-6684
Launched : 2013
JSM Environmental Science and Ecology
ISSN : 2333-7141
Launched : 2013
Journal of Cardiology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2333-6676
Launched : 2013
JSM Nanotechnology and Nanomedicine
ISSN : 2334-1815
Launched : 2013
Journal of Ear, Nose and Throat Disorders
ISSN : 2475-9473
Launched : 2016
JSM Ophthalmology
ISSN : 2333-6447
Launched : 2013
Journal of Pharmacology and Clinical Toxicology
ISSN : 2333-7079
Launched : 2013
Annals of Psychiatry and Mental Health
ISSN : 2374-0124
Launched : 2013
Medical Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology
ISSN : 2333-6439
Launched : 2013
Annals of Pediatrics and Child Health
ISSN : 2373-9312
Launched : 2013
JSM Clinical Pharmaceutics
ISSN : 2379-9498
Launched : 2014
JSM Foot and Ankle
ISSN : 2475-9112
Launched : 2016
JSM Alzheimer's Disease and Related Dementia
ISSN : 2378-9565
Launched : 2014
Journal of Addiction Medicine and Therapy
ISSN : 2333-665X
Launched : 2013
Journal of Veterinary Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2378-931X
Launched : 2013
Annals of Public Health and Research
ISSN : 2378-9328
Launched : 2014
Annals of Orthopedics and Rheumatology
ISSN : 2373-9290
Launched : 2013
Journal of Clinical Nephrology and Research
ISSN : 2379-0652
Launched : 2014
Annals of Community Medicine and Practice
ISSN : 2475-9465
Launched : 2014
Annals of Biometrics and Biostatistics
ISSN : 2374-0116
Launched : 2013
JSM Clinical Case Reports
ISSN : 2373-9819
Launched : 2013
Journal of Cancer Biology and Research
ISSN : 2373-9436
Launched : 2013
Journal of Surgery and Transplantation Science
ISSN : 2379-0911
Launched : 2013
Journal of Dermatology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2373-9371
Launched : 2013
JSM Gastroenterology and Hepatology
ISSN : 2373-9487
Launched : 2013
Annals of Nursing and Practice
ISSN : 2379-9501
Launched : 2014
JSM Dentistry
ISSN : 2333-7133
Launched : 2013
Author Information X