Loading

Journal of Neurological Disorders and Stroke

The Effect of Saccades to Distractions on the Reaction Time in ADHD-Inattentive Subtype and Normal Children

Research Article | Open Access | Volume 6 | Issue 1

  • 1. 1 Department of Biomedical Engineering, Amirkabir University of Technology, Iran
  • 2. Department of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of Tehran, Iran
+ Show More - Show Less
Corresponding Authors
Farzad Towhidkhah, Department of Biomedical Engineering, Amirkabir University of Technology, 424 Hafez Ave, Tehran, Iran, Tel: 98-2164-542-363
ABSTRACT

The negative effect of distractions on the individuals’ attention performance and the possible relation between the performance of the attention system and the pattern of saccadic eye movements has been investigated in different studies. It has been shown that distraction can lead to a delayed saccade toward a target and consequently the increment of the reaction time. The abnormal distractibility of subjects with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder has also been reported in previous studies. In the current study, we investigated the relationship between the saccadic eye movement to the distractions and speed of the response in children with attention deficit disorder (ADD) and normal ones.

All children participated in a visual attention experiment. Subjects were requested to respond to a target when a target and a distraction were presented. Saccadic eye movements and RTs were recorded during the test. Paired samples t-test was used to compare the RT in trials with and without unwanted eye movements (saccade to distraction).

Statistical analysis showed that unwanted saccades led to a significant increase of the RT in normal children (p.05). It was also shown that the normal group had lower RTs than children with ADD in both trials with and without saccades to the distractions (p< 0.001).

The outcomes of this study provided some suggestions about the difference between overt and covert attention to the distractions in normal and ADD children and new diagnostic methods have been proposed for future works.

CITATION

Baghdadi G, Towhidkhah F, Rostami R (2018) The Effect of Saccades to Distractions on the Reaction Time in ADHD-Inattentive Subtype and Normal Children. J Neurol Disord Stroke 6(1): 1137.

KEYWORDS

• Reaction time; Attention deficit disorder; Covert attention; Overt attention; Distraction

ABBREVIATIONS

ADHD: Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder; ADD: Attention Deficit Disorder; EEG: Electroencephalography; MMN: Mismatch Negativity; ERP: Event-Related Potential; RON: ReOrientation of Negativity; RT: Reaction Time; RDE: Remote Distractor Effect

INTRODUCTION

In neurocognitive science, distraction refers to any things that are not related to the goals of a target task and can divert the individuals’ mind from the goals. Distractions compete with the target stimuli to use the limited neurocognitive processing resources [1]. The neurocognitive system compares the incoming stimuli with the predefined goals to dissociate distractions from target stimuli. Earlier perception of a stimulus and detection of its features takes about 150 ms after its presentation, and its comparison with the predefined target stimulus finishes after about 250 ms [2].

Behavioral analyses have shown that distractions can reduce the speed and the accuracy of responses. It has been believed that the speed reduction is not due to the slower processing of target stimuli. In other words, the distraction does not interfere with the processing of the target stimuli. The reason for the speed reduction is attributed to the time required to orient the processing resources from the target to the distraction and the initiation of involuntary procedures. Brain electrical activities (i.e., electroencephalography (EEG)) demonstrate that by the presentation of an unexpected distraction, attention control system detects it and as a result of this detection the mismatch negativity (MMN) wave is usually produced in the event-related potential (ERP) signal [3,4]. Then, processing resources are usually involuntary oriented towards the unexpected distraction to process in further that leads to the increment of the P300 amplitude. After a while, the processing resources are reoriented towards the previous ongoing target task. This reorientation is accompanied by the production of the Re-orientation of negativity (RON) component in the ERP signals. These events have been observed in both synchronous and asynchronous presentations of target and distraction stimuli [4].

If the attention control system cannot detect and attenuate distractions timely and correctly, the performance of the individual may decline during the task.

The weakness of the attention control system in inhibiting the distractions has been reported in some of the disorders such as the attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), which is one of the neurodevelopmental disorders among children. 

This disorder has three inattentive, hyperactive, and combined subtypes. The mentioned weakness is more often reported in the inattentive that is also called attention deficit disorder (ADD). These children are more sensitive to distractions in comparison with normal ones [5-8]. That is, by the presentation of distractions, the performance decline in ADD subjects is higher than the normal group. In ADD subjects, the deficit of the attention control system to the correct and on-time attenuation of distraction is attributed to the weakness of the working memory [9].

The mentioned deviation of the processing resources to the distraction may be followed by the eye movement (e.g., saccade) towards the distractor, which is called overt attention to the distraction. The second possible condition is only accompanied by the deviation of processing resources and no movement towards the distraction is observed, which is called covert attention [10]. The characteristics of eye movements were investigated in ADHD and normal subjects [11-17]. It was shown that ADHD children had problem in gaze fixations. This problem was attributed to the late maturation of the frontal cortex in ADHD children [11]. It was also reported that that people with ADHD had higher saccadic reaction times and errors in anti-saccades than those of normal group [12,13]. In another study, it was demonstrated that ADHD subjects needed more time to inhibit the unexpected eye movements [15]. These problems were attributed to the late maturation of the frontal cortex in ADHD children [11], specifically the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex [14].

The possible relation between the performance of the attention system (usually assessed by measuring the reaction time (RT) and the pattern of saccadic eye movements has been investigated in different studies [18,19]. It has been shown that distraction may lead to a delayed saccade to the target [20,21]. Problems in the inhibition of saccadic eye movements to distractions were also reported in the previous studies [16,17].

The main goal of the current study is to find out whether saccadic eye movements to the distractions have a considerable effect on the speed of the response (i.e., reaction time (RT) in both ADHD and normal group, or not?

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Participants

Forty-two normal (25girls; 9 ± 0.6 years old) and seventeen ADD (8 girls; 9.4 ± 0.8 years old) IQ matched (using Goodenouph test) children participated in the experiment. Children with ADD were diagnosed by psychiatrists in the Atieh comprehensive psychiatric center of Iran based on DSM-IV, IVA CPT, and QEEG indices. These children did not use any medication and did not receive any behavioral interventions. The parents of normal children were requested to fill the national institute for children’s health quality (NICHQ) Vanderbilt assessment scales to evaluate the mental health of this group. Participants had normal vision.

Before the experiment, children and their parents were informed about the test procedures and the parents signed the informed consent forms. The experiment was done under the approval of Iran University of Medical Sciences (# IR.IUMS. REC.1395 90133916).

Experiment

The experiment was a short time visual attention task. It consisted of 38 trials, which the first six trials were considered as a practice to warm up the participant. In each trial, a target (a star shown in Figure 1 (a)) and a distractor (an image of a colored or black and white fruit shown in Figure 1 (b)) were presented. Subjects were requested to tap on a touchpad as soon as seeing a star (presented for 400ms) in the predefined target locations on a monitor. The images of the distractors were shown in the predetermined non-target locations, 200ms before the presentation of the star. The predefined position of targets and distractors locations is shown in Figure 2 respectively by circles and squares. The height of both target and distraction stimuli was about one inch. That is, participants were informed that in each trial the star is presented in one of the circles and distractors presented in one of the squares and they were requested to pay no attention to the position of the squares. Saccadic eye movements and RTs were recorded during the test.

Participants sat in front of a screen with a distance of about 70 cm. The screen was a 21” LCD screen with the resolution of 1600×900 pixels and the refresh rate of 60 Hz. During the test, a camera (1.2 MP, 30 fps) was placed under the monitor in front of the subject’s face. A program in the Visual C# 2008 software running under Microsoft windows 7 controlled the presentation and storage of camera output and subjects responses. This program labeled the data recorded by the camera at the start and end of each trial. The frames of different trials were extracted from the videos recorded by the camera. Comparing the position of eyes in the successive frames, the direction of eye movements was detected. The positions of distractions and targets were known in each trial. Therefore, according to the extracted eye movements’ directions, saccades to the distractions were detected. These processes (i.e., eye movement analyses) were done offline (i.e., at the end of the experiment).

Measurements and statistical test

Paired samples t-test was used to compare the RT in trials with and without unwanted eye movements (saccade to distraction).

The reaction times of the participants were recorded during the test. To evaluate the difference between recorded reaction times in trials with a saccade to the distraction and those without the saccade, a paired-t-test was used. The differences between the normal and ADD groups were examined by an unpaired-t-test with a significant level of 0.05.

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION

To find out the effect of saccadic eye movement on the RT, the trials that the participants had a saccade to the location of the distraction was separated from trials with no saccade to the distractor. Then, the average RTs of these two kinds of trials was compared in both normal and ADD groups.

Using unpaired-t-test, a significant difference was found between ADD and normal groups in both trials with (t(57) = 2.6, p = .01) and without (t(57) = 4, p = .0002) saccades to the distractions. These results were demonstrated in Figure 3 and were summarized in Table 1.

As mentioned in the introduction, distraction can lead to a delayed response to the target. The underlying causes of this delay are not completely known. However, two possibilities have been suggested for the observed delay: 1-The time of choosing the correct location for attending, and 2-The time required for the processing of distractor properties (called “remote distractor effect” (RDE))[20, 21].

Target locations were predefined in our experiment. Therefore, the increase of the RT cannot be due to the making decision about the target position. For that reason, the increase of the RT can be as a result of the distractor characteristics processing (or RDE). Based on the statistical analysis, it can also be claimed that the difference between ADD and normal children in trials without saccades to the distractors was stronger than situations with a saccade to the distractions. This observation suggests that the differentiation between ADD and normal children in covert attention is more than the overt attention, based on the negative effect of RDE.

Statistical analyses (paired-t-test) also showed that, in the normal group, the values of RTs in trials with a saccade to the distraction was significantly higher than the trials without saccades to the distractions (t(41) = 3.026, p = .004). This result is consistent with the claim that for better processing of a target it is required to be in the center of the field of the view [18,19]. When the individuals have a saccade to the distraction, the position of his/her eyes is replaced towards the distraction. As a result, a disturbance and a possible delay may occur in the processing of the target that may lead to the higher RTs.

However, in ADD children, there was no significant difference between the mentioned two kinds of trials (t(16) = 0.6, p = .6). This result is inconsistent with all the above interpretations provided for the normal children observations. It means that there was no significant difference between the covert or overt attention to the distraction in ADD children. By a comparison between the results obtained for the normal and ADD groups, it can be suggested that normal children can inhibit their covert attention to the distractions. Therefore, if they have no involuntary saccades to the distraction (i.e., overt attention), they can attenuate the negative effect of distraction. However, in ADD children, even if the involuntary saccades to the distraction (i.e., overt attention) are inhibited, the covert attention to the saccade cannot be controlled correctly due to their weakness of the attention control system. The control of covert distraction is done by the execution and inhibitory systems. The weakness of these systems can lead to the abnormal covert or overt distractions. Overt distraction can be detected by tracking the eye movements. However, the detection of covert distraction is not easily possible. It has been believed that unexpected increment of the reaction time or the decline of the activities of brain regions that involved in the target processing may be due to the covert distraction. Therefore, to investigate the differences between ADD and normal subjects from the aspect of covert and overt attention performance, both eye movement recording and reaction times (or brain activities) are required. This results is consistent with previous studies on the relationship between eye movements’ patterns, the deficit of inhibitory system and executive function, and ADHD symptoms [16,17].

Table 1: Results of statistical comparisons between normal and ADD children in trials with and without saccades to the distraction.

Feature Normal children ADD children  
Number 42 17  
Sex 25 girls; 17 boys 8 girls; 9 boys  
Age 9 ± 0.6 years old 9.4 ± 0.8 years  
  Stats (p=0.05)
t(df)=t-stat, p=p-value
RT in trials with saccade to the distraction (mean ± SEM)
635.5 ± 20.21
(mean ± SEM)
730.4 ± 28.12
t(57) = 2.6, p = .01
RT in trials with NO saccade to the distraction 606.8 ± 15.79 747.6 ± 38.73 t(57) = 4, p = .0002
Stats (p=0.05)
t(df)=t-stat, p=p-value
t(41) = 3.026, p = .004 t(16) = 0.6, p = .6  

 

CONCLUSION

Our experiment showed that the saccadic eye movements can affect the distractor processing and consequently, the children attention performance. It was also shown that this effect is different in ADD and normal children. The available common clinical computer-based diagnosis methods do not include the presentation of distraction during the task. According to the results of this study and other similar ones, adding several levels of distractions and some criteria to evaluate the distractibility may enhance the accuracy of the diagnostic methods. The current study also showed the difference between the effect of overt and covert attention to the distractions in ADHD and normal children. Therefore, developing new methods that can differentiate the influence of overt and covert attention is suggested for future works to have more accurate and faster diagnosis of ADHD. For example, instead of common diagnostic methods (e.g., test of variables of attention, or integrated visual and auditory test) that usually take a long time and lead to the lack of cooperation between the children and the experimenter, new shorter tests that contain distractions are suggested. According to the results, distractions excite the overt attention involuntary and this effect is different between normal and ADHD children. To develop these methods a system that can track the subjects’ saccades to the presented distractions is needed. The relationship between the pattern of these saccades and recorded reaction times can show the performance of overt attention. Comparing this performance in normal and ADHD children can be used as new diagnostic criteria.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

This research has been supported by the cognitive sciences and technologies council grant (#201).

REFERENCES

1. Lavie N. Attention, distraction, and cognitive control under load. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2010; 19: 143-148.

2. Bansal AK, Madhavan R, Agam Y, Golby A, Madsen JR, Kreiman G. Neural dynamics underlying target detection in the human brain. J Neurosci. 2014; 34: 3042-3055.

3. Fritz JB, Elhilali M, David SV, Shamma SA. Auditory attention--focusing the searchlight on sound. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2007; 17: 437-455.

4. Parmentier FB, Ljungberg JK, Elsley JV, Lindkvist M. A behavioral study of distraction by vibrotactile novelty. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2011; 37: 1134.

5. van Mourik R, Oosterlaan J, Heslenfeld DJ, Konig CE, Sergeant JA. When distraction is not distracting: A behavioral and ERP study on distraction in ADHD. Clin Neurophysiol. 2007; 118: 1855-1865.

6. Franklin MS, Mrazek MD, Anderson CL, Johnston C, Smallwood J, Kingstone A, et al. Tracking distraction: The relationship between mind-wandering, meta-awareness, and ADHD symptomatology. J Atten Disord. 2017; 21: 475-486.

7. Adams R, Finn P, Moes E, Flannery K, Rizzo AS. Distractibility in attention/deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): The virtual reality classroom. Child Neuropsychol. 2009; 15: 120-135.

8. Reimer B, Mehler B, D’Ambrosio LA, Fried R. The impact of distractions on young adult drivers with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Accid Anal Prev. 2010; 42: 842-851.

9. Parmentier FB, Elford G, Escera C, Andrés P, San Miguel I. The cognitive locus of distraction by acoustic novelty in the cross-modal oddball task. Cognition. 2008; 106: 408-432.

10. Itti L, Koch C. A saliency-based search mechanism for overt and covert shifts of visual attention. Vision research. 2000; 40: 1489-1506.

11. Damyanovich E, Baziyan BK, Sagalov M, Kumskova G. Saccadic movements of the eyes in children with attention deficit and hyperactivity syndrome. Bull Exp Biol Med. 2013; 156: 25-28.

12. Schwerdtfeger RMH, Alahyane N, Brien DC, Coe BC, Stroman PW, Munoz DP. Preparatory neural networks are impaired in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder during the antisaccade task. Neuroimage Clin. 2013; 2: 63-78.

13. Munoz DP, Armstrong IT, Hampton KA, Moore KD. Altered control of visual fixation and saccadic eye movements in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Neurophysiol. 2003; 90: 503-514.

14. Ross RG, Hommer D, Breiger D, Varley C, Radant A. Eye movement task related to frontal lobe functioning in children with attention deficit disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1994; 33: 869-874.

15. Fillmore MT, Milich R, Lorch EP. Inhibitory deficits in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Intentional versus automatic mechanisms of attention. Dev Psychopathol. 2009; 21: 539-54.

16. West GL, Mendizabal S, Carrière M-P, Lippé S. Linear age-correlated development of inhibitory saccadic trajectory deviations. Dev Psychol. 2014; 50: 2285-2290.

17. West GL, Lippé S. The development of inhibitory saccadic trajectory deviations correlates with measures of antisaccadic inhibition. Neuroreport. 2016; 27: 1196-1201.

18. Rolfs M, Jonikaitis D, Deubel H, Cavanagh P. Predictive remapping of attention across eye movements. Nat Neurosci. 2011; 14: 252-256.

19. Zhao M, Gersch TM, Schnitzer BS, Dosher BA, Kowler E. Eye movements and attention: The role of pre-saccadic shifts of attention in perception, memory and the control of saccades. Vision Res. 2012; 74: 40-60.

20. Bompas A, Sumner P. Saccadic inhibition and the remote distractor effect: One mechanism or two? J Vis. 2015; 15: 15.

21. McIntosh RD, Buonocore A. Saccadic inhibition can cause the remote distractor effect, but the remote distractor effect may not be a useful concept. J Vis. 2014; 14: 15.

Baghdadi G, Towhidkhah F, Rostami R (2018) The Effect of Saccades to Distractions on the Reaction Time in ADHD-Inattentive Subtype and Normal Children. J Neurol Disord Stroke 6(1): 1137.

Received : 04 Apr 2018
Accepted : 27 Apr 2018
Published : 30 Apr 2018
Journals
Annals of Otolaryngology and Rhinology
ISSN : 2379-948X
Launched : 2014
JSM Schizophrenia
Launched : 2016
Journal of Nausea
Launched : 2020
JSM Internal Medicine
Launched : 2016
JSM Hepatitis
Launched : 2016
JSM Oro Facial Surgeries
ISSN : 2578-3211
Launched : 2016
Journal of Human Nutrition and Food Science
ISSN : 2333-6706
Launched : 2013
JSM Regenerative Medicine and Bioengineering
ISSN : 2379-0490
Launched : 2013
JSM Spine
ISSN : 2578-3181
Launched : 2016
Archives of Palliative Care
ISSN : 2573-1165
Launched : 2016
JSM Nutritional Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3203
Launched : 2017
Annals of Neurodegenerative Disorders
ISSN : 2476-2032
Launched : 2016
Journal of Fever
ISSN : 2641-7782
Launched : 2017
JSM Bone Marrow Research
ISSN : 2578-3351
Launched : 2016
JSM Mathematics and Statistics
ISSN : 2578-3173
Launched : 2014
Journal of Autoimmunity and Research
ISSN : 2573-1173
Launched : 2014
JSM Arthritis
ISSN : 2475-9155
Launched : 2016
JSM Head and Neck Cancer-Cases and Reviews
ISSN : 2573-1610
Launched : 2016
JSM General Surgery Cases and Images
ISSN : 2573-1564
Launched : 2016
JSM Anatomy and Physiology
ISSN : 2573-1262
Launched : 2016
JSM Dental Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1548
Launched : 2016
Annals of Emergency Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1017
Launched : 2016
Annals of Mens Health and Wellness
ISSN : 2641-7707
Launched : 2017
Journal of Preventive Medicine and Health Care
ISSN : 2576-0084
Launched : 2018
Journal of Chronic Diseases and Management
ISSN : 2573-1300
Launched : 2016
Annals of Vaccines and Immunization
ISSN : 2378-9379
Launched : 2014
JSM Heart Surgery Cases and Images
ISSN : 2578-3157
Launched : 2016
Annals of Reproductive Medicine and Treatment
ISSN : 2573-1092
Launched : 2016
JSM Brain Science
ISSN : 2573-1289
Launched : 2016
JSM Biomarkers
ISSN : 2578-3815
Launched : 2014
JSM Biology
ISSN : 2475-9392
Launched : 2016
Archives of Stem Cell and Research
ISSN : 2578-3580
Launched : 2014
Annals of Clinical and Medical Microbiology
ISSN : 2578-3629
Launched : 2014
JSM Pediatric Surgery
ISSN : 2578-3149
Launched : 2017
Journal of Memory Disorder and Rehabilitation
ISSN : 2578-319X
Launched : 2016
JSM Tropical Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2578-3165
Launched : 2016
JSM Head and Face Medicine
ISSN : 2578-3793
Launched : 2016
JSM Cardiothoracic Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1297
Launched : 2016
JSM Bone and Joint Diseases
ISSN : 2578-3351
Launched : 2017
JSM Bioavailability and Bioequivalence
ISSN : 2641-7812
Launched : 2017
JSM Atherosclerosis
ISSN : 2573-1270
Launched : 2016
Journal of Genitourinary Disorders
ISSN : 2641-7790
Launched : 2017
Journal of Fractures and Sprains
ISSN : 2578-3831
Launched : 2016
Journal of Autism and Epilepsy
ISSN : 2641-7774
Launched : 2016
Annals of Marine Biology and Research
ISSN : 2573-105X
Launched : 2014
JSM Health Education & Primary Health Care
ISSN : 2578-3777
Launched : 2016
JSM Communication Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3807
Launched : 2016
Annals of Musculoskeletal Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3599
Launched : 2016
Annals of Virology and Research
ISSN : 2573-1122
Launched : 2014
JSM Renal Medicine
ISSN : 2573-1637
Launched : 2016
Journal of Muscle Health
ISSN : 2578-3823
Launched : 2016
JSM Genetics and Genomics
ISSN : 2334-1823
Launched : 2013
JSM Anxiety and Depression
ISSN : 2475-9139
Launched : 2016
Clinical Journal of Heart Diseases
ISSN : 2641-7766
Launched : 2016
Annals of Medicinal Chemistry and Research
ISSN : 2378-9336
Launched : 2014
JSM Pain and Management
ISSN : 2578-3378
Launched : 2016
JSM Women's Health
ISSN : 2578-3696
Launched : 2016
Clinical Research in HIV or AIDS
ISSN : 2374-0094
Launched : 2013
Journal of Endocrinology, Diabetes and Obesity
ISSN : 2333-6692
Launched : 2013
Journal of Substance Abuse and Alcoholism
ISSN : 2373-9363
Launched : 2013
JSM Neurosurgery and Spine
ISSN : 2373-9479
Launched : 2013
Journal of Liver and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2379-0830
Launched : 2014
Journal of Drug Design and Research
ISSN : 2379-089X
Launched : 2014
JSM Clinical Oncology and Research
ISSN : 2373-938X
Launched : 2013
JSM Bioinformatics, Genomics and Proteomics
ISSN : 2576-1102
Launched : 2014
JSM Chemistry
ISSN : 2334-1831
Launched : 2013
Journal of Trauma and Care
ISSN : 2573-1246
Launched : 2014
JSM Surgical Oncology and Research
ISSN : 2578-3688
Launched : 2016
Annals of Food Processing and Preservation
ISSN : 2573-1033
Launched : 2016
Journal of Radiology and Radiation Therapy
ISSN : 2333-7095
Launched : 2013
JSM Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation
ISSN : 2578-3572
Launched : 2016
Annals of Clinical Pathology
ISSN : 2373-9282
Launched : 2013
Annals of Cardiovascular Diseases
ISSN : 2641-7731
Launched : 2016
Journal of Behavior
ISSN : 2576-0076
Launched : 2016
Annals of Clinical and Experimental Metabolism
ISSN : 2572-2492
Launched : 2016
Clinical Research in Infectious Diseases
ISSN : 2379-0636
Launched : 2013
JSM Microbiology
ISSN : 2333-6455
Launched : 2013
Journal of Urology and Research
ISSN : 2379-951X
Launched : 2014
Journal of Family Medicine and Community Health
ISSN : 2379-0547
Launched : 2013
Annals of Pregnancy and Care
ISSN : 2578-336X
Launched : 2017
JSM Cell and Developmental Biology
ISSN : 2379-061X
Launched : 2013
Annals of Aquaculture and Research
ISSN : 2379-0881
Launched : 2014
Clinical Research in Pulmonology
ISSN : 2333-6625
Launched : 2013
Journal of Immunology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2333-6714
Launched : 2013
Annals of Forensic Research and Analysis
ISSN : 2378-9476
Launched : 2014
JSM Biochemistry and Molecular Biology
ISSN : 2333-7109
Launched : 2013
Annals of Breast Cancer Research
ISSN : 2641-7685
Launched : 2016
Annals of Gerontology and Geriatric Research
ISSN : 2378-9409
Launched : 2014
Journal of Sleep Medicine and Disorders
ISSN : 2379-0822
Launched : 2014
JSM Burns and Trauma
ISSN : 2475-9406
Launched : 2016
Chemical Engineering and Process Techniques
ISSN : 2333-6633
Launched : 2013
Annals of Clinical Cytology and Pathology
ISSN : 2475-9430
Launched : 2014
JSM Allergy and Asthma
ISSN : 2573-1254
Launched : 2016
Annals of Sports Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2379-0571
Launched : 2014
JSM Sexual Medicine
ISSN : 2578-3718
Launched : 2016
Annals of Vascular Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2378-9344
Launched : 2014
JSM Biotechnology and Biomedical Engineering
ISSN : 2333-7117
Launched : 2013
Journal of Hematology and Transfusion
ISSN : 2333-6684
Launched : 2013
JSM Environmental Science and Ecology
ISSN : 2333-7141
Launched : 2013
Journal of Cardiology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2333-6676
Launched : 2013
JSM Nanotechnology and Nanomedicine
ISSN : 2334-1815
Launched : 2013
Journal of Ear, Nose and Throat Disorders
ISSN : 2475-9473
Launched : 2016
JSM Ophthalmology
ISSN : 2333-6447
Launched : 2013
Journal of Pharmacology and Clinical Toxicology
ISSN : 2333-7079
Launched : 2013
Annals of Psychiatry and Mental Health
ISSN : 2374-0124
Launched : 2013
Medical Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology
ISSN : 2333-6439
Launched : 2013
Annals of Pediatrics and Child Health
ISSN : 2373-9312
Launched : 2013
JSM Clinical Pharmaceutics
ISSN : 2379-9498
Launched : 2014
JSM Foot and Ankle
ISSN : 2475-9112
Launched : 2016
JSM Alzheimer's Disease and Related Dementia
ISSN : 2378-9565
Launched : 2014
Journal of Addiction Medicine and Therapy
ISSN : 2333-665X
Launched : 2013
Journal of Veterinary Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2378-931X
Launched : 2013
Annals of Public Health and Research
ISSN : 2378-9328
Launched : 2014
Annals of Orthopedics and Rheumatology
ISSN : 2373-9290
Launched : 2013
Journal of Clinical Nephrology and Research
ISSN : 2379-0652
Launched : 2014
Annals of Community Medicine and Practice
ISSN : 2475-9465
Launched : 2014
Annals of Biometrics and Biostatistics
ISSN : 2374-0116
Launched : 2013
JSM Clinical Case Reports
ISSN : 2373-9819
Launched : 2013
Journal of Cancer Biology and Research
ISSN : 2373-9436
Launched : 2013
Journal of Surgery and Transplantation Science
ISSN : 2379-0911
Launched : 2013
Journal of Dermatology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2373-9371
Launched : 2013
JSM Gastroenterology and Hepatology
ISSN : 2373-9487
Launched : 2013
Annals of Nursing and Practice
ISSN : 2379-9501
Launched : 2014
JSM Dentistry
ISSN : 2333-7133
Launched : 2013
Author Information X