Loading

Journal of Substance Abuse and Alcoholism

Alcohol-Attentional Bias in Alcohol-Dependent and Cocaine-Dependent Patients

Research Article | Open Access | Volume 2 | Issue 2

  • 1. Department of Psychiatry, Laboratory of Clinical Psychophysiology, Hospital Universitario 12 de Octubre, Spain
  • 2. Department of Psychiatry, Hospital Universitario 12 de Octubre, Spain
  • 3. 3Biomedical Research Center Network for Mental Health, Spain
  • 4. Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Complutense University of Madrid, Spain
  • 5. 5 Department of Basic Psychology II, School of Psychology, Complutense University of Madrid, Spain
  • 6. Department of Basic Psychology I, Complutense University of Madrid, Spain
  • 7. 7 Thematic Network of Cooperative Health Research, Institute of Health Carlos III, Spain
  • 8. Center of Biomedical Technology, Laboratory of Cognitive and Computational Neuroscience, Laboratory of Clinical Neuroscience, Technical University of Madrid, Spain
+ Show More - Show Less
Corresponding Authors
Isabel Morales Munoz, Laboratory of Clinical Psychophysiology, Psychiatry Service, Hospital Universitario 12 de Octubre, Avda Córdoba s/n, 28045 Madrid, Spain Tel: 34-91-390-8019
ABSTRACT

Background: Substance-related attentional bias refers to the reactivity to substance-related cues. This attentional bias to drugs has been examined in different addictive disorders such as cocaine, alcohol or tobacco dependence. There is extensive evidence regarding the attentional bias to alcohol-related cues in Alcohol Dependent (AD) patients. Furthermore, there is evidence regarding the higher attention bias to cocaine-related cues in Cocaine Dependent (CD) subjects after the exposure to alcohol consumption. However, there are still no data on the potential attentional bias to alcohol-related cues in patients diagnosed with CD.

Objectives: we aimed to assess attentional bias in a sample of alcohol and cocaine users with a visual probe task.

Material and methods: We used a sample of 35 AD patients, 30 CD patients and a control group formed by 35 healthy volunteers. Moreover, and to further study alcohol attentional bias in CD subjects, we divided this group in terms of their history of alcohol consumption. All subjects were examined using the visual probe task, in order to study the attentional bias to alcohol-related cues.

Results: The patients that showed the greater attentional bias to alcohol-related cues were the AD subjects, followed by the CD patients and finally by controls. AD and CD exhibited lower reaction times to alcohol- congruent condition compared to the alcohol-incongruent, whereas in controls the opposite effect was found.

Discussion: Our results indicated that although attentional bias to alcohol-related cues was clearly found in AD and CD patients, these data are in accordance with the hypothesis about the fact that cocaine dependence increases the attentional bias to other drugs, such as alcohol.

KEYWORDS

• Attentional bias
• Alcohol dependence
• Cocaine dependence
• Visual probe task

CITATION

Morales-Muñoz I, Puras P, Rigabert A, Álvarez-Alonso MJ, et al. (2014) Alcohol-Attentional Bias in Alcohol-Dependent and Cocaine-Dependent Patients. J Subst Abuse Alcohol 2(2): 1013.

ABBREVIATIONS

AD: Alcohol Dependence; CD: Cocaine Dependence; VPT: Visual Probe Task; US: Unconditioned Stimuli; UR: Unconditioned Response; CS: Conditioned Stimuli; CR: Conditioned Response

INTRODUCTION

Addictive behaviors are frequently associated with reactivity to substance-related cues, also known as substance-related attentional bias [1]. In the study of addiction, attentional bias refers to the observation that substance-related cues capture the attention of experienced substance users [2], and this is based on the theory that the substance and its associated cues increase motivational incentive and salience with each new administration [3,4]. As is commonly understood [3], substances of abuse can operate as Unconditioned Stimuli (US) that elicits Unconditioned Responses (UR). Through conditioning a substance can become associated with environmental stimuli that are contiguous with and contingent on the effects of that substance. That is, after repeated pairings of US with a Conditioned Stimulus (CS), the CS comes to elicit a Conditioned Response (CR), such as physiological arousal or craving to use the substance. One consequence of classical conditioning is cues orient attention towards a predictive CS when encountered [5-7]. Therefore, when conditioning occurs and a substance of abuse is the US, one would expect that substance-related stimuli would attract the user’s attention [3].

Researchers have found attentional bias for substance-related stimuli (presented verbally, pictorially, or as in vivo exposure) in users of different substances such as cocaine [8], alcohol [9] and tobacco [10]. Alcohol ingestion, for instance, increases this attentional bias to alcohol-associated stimuli [11]. In heavy social drinkers, alcohol- associated stimuli grab attention [1,12] and increase both the urge to drink alcohol and the amount of alcohol intake However, previous studies in individuals with alcohol dependence have shown that, unlike social drinking controls, individuals with alcohol dependence exhibit avoidance for alcohol-related visual stimuli in a dot probe detection task. Avoidance suggests the finding of a negative bias whereby the patients allocate their attention to the control stimuli and away from the alcohol-related stimuli [12]. Nikolaou et al found that attentional bias to alcohol related stimuli was reliably observed at the low, but not at the high alcohol dose when compared with placebo [13]. Thus, despite there are several studies related to attentional bias in alcohol, the underlying cognitive processes remain unknown.

Alcohol and cocaine are used in higher quantities with concomitant use than when either of the substances are used individually [14], and it is proven by a wide variety of researches that cocaine and alcohol combination produces additive psychological and physiological effects [15]. Therefore, and given that dopamine activity in the mesolimbic pathway is hypothesised to be responsible for incentive salience attribution and attentional bias [3,16], acute administration of drugs that increase dopaminergic activity should lead to increases in attentional bias for any drug-related cues. In addition, Montgomery et al found increased attentional bias for cocaine cues following preload with alcohol in regular cocaine users, by means of a visual probe task [17]. In cocaine attentional bias, researches using the cocaine emotional Stroop task showed that subjects who use cocaine had slower reaction time compared to controls [8]. Other authors also observed that regions implicated in the general orientation of attention also showed significantly increased activation under low load in presence of the alcohol-associated stimuli compared to the neutral stimuli, and this observation extends to those findings with cocaine-related stimuli [17]. Now then, if cocaine consumption is associated with alcohol consumption in clinical samples and in general population [18], it can be hypothesized that cocaine may increase attentional bias alcohol-related cues in subjects without any alcohol use disorder (e.g., abuse or dependence).

The present study sought to assess attentional bias in a sample of alcohol and cocaine users with a visual probe task. We hypothesized that Alcohol Dependent (AD) patients and cocainedependent subjects would show greater attentional bias for alcohol stimuli compared to controls.

METHODS

Subjects

A sample of 100 individuals was recruited for the study. Groups were divided into 35 AD patients, 35 CD patients and a control group formed by 35 healthy volunteers. In the AD group (26males/9females), the average age was 47.20 years (SD=1.26); the average age for the CD group (30males/5females) was 42.63 years (SD=7.162) and within the control group (23males/12females) the average age was 43.26 years (SD=1.34).

All AD and CD patients underwent detoxification before assessment for this study, and were referred for treatment from Primary Care and Mental Health centres. Patients included fulfilled DSM-IV criteria for diagnosis of alcohol or cocaine dependence (DSM-IV-TR) [19], determined using the validated Spanish version of the Psychiatric Research Interview for Substance and Mental Disorders (PRISM) [20]. Exclusion criteria for patients were current drug abuse other than alcohol, cocaine or nicotine, and any medication that could influence psychophysiological parameters. In the CD group, patients who also fulfilled alcohol dependence criteria were also excluded. Five subjects from the initial 35 CD patients were finally rejected, in order to control possible confounding variables in these patients. Therefore, the final sample of CD patients was composed of 30 subjects. All the patients were abstinent from alcohol and/ or cocaine for more than one month (a mean of 35 days). This abstinence was confirmed by toxicological analyses every three days. Patients and controls performed a urine drug test before the task-dot session.

Regarding controls, they were recruited from the local community and from the mental health service staff. Before inclusion, a semi-structured interview was used to screen control subjects for exclusion criteria: thyroid dysfunction, heart disease, hypotension or hypertension, regular drug prescriptions, a history of mental illness (psychotic disorders, bipolar disorders, panic and generalized anxiety disorders), rapid mood changes, drug (determined by urine toxicology screening) and alcohol abuse, and alcohol dependence in first-degree relatives. Subjects taking psychotropic medications were excluded from the study.

The local ethics committee approved the study. All patients gave written, informed consent.

METHODS

Clinical assessment

For the clinical assessment, the following scales were used:

- The Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HDRS) [21]

This scale is a multiple item questionnaire used to provide an indication of depression. This questionnaire is designed for adults and is used to rate the severity of their depression by probing mood, feelings of guilt, suicide ideation, insomnia, agitation or retardation, anxiety, weight loss, and somatic symptoms.

- Severity of Alcohol Dependence Scale (IADS) [22]

The IADS is a self-administered questionnaire designed for the assessment of the severity of alcohol dependence. This scale is composed of 30 items subdivided into 6 subscales: symptoms of physical and psychological abstinence; behaviors for the relief of abstinence; regular alcohol consumption; difficulties for alcohol control; and symptomatology restoration after relapse.

- The Barrat Impulsiveness Scale (BIS) [23]

The BIS is a questionnaire designed to assess the personality/ behavioral construct of impulsiveness. This scale consists on 30 items and it is scored to yield a total score, three second-order factors (attentional, motor and non- planning), and six first-order factors (attention, motor, self-control, cognitive complexity, perseverance and cognitive instability).

Psychophysiological assessment

For the psychophysiological assessment, we used the Visual Probe Task (VPT), which was based on the Lubman [24] task. In this task, twenty-seven alcohol-related pictures (“cue pictures”) were matched to 27 non-alcohol-related pictures (“neutral pictures”). Cue pictures included different types of alcohol stimuli, such as a can of beer, a bottle of alcohol, a glass of wine, a brewery….To select the cue pictures, we initially preselected 40 alcohol-related images available in “Google Images”. From those initial 40 pictures, we selected those with the most motivational relevance [25]. To do this, we administered the Self-Assessment Manikin Visual Analogue Scale (SAM-VAS) to a sample of 20 subjects with AD diagnosis and in 20 subjects without any psychiatry diagnosis as assessed by the PRISM [19]. From the 40 preselected images, 27 pictures had a score on the SAM-VAS scale larger than the mean and were selected as the cues for this study. To find a matching neutral image, another search was performed in Google Images for each cue picture. Pictures were matched on shape, size, and color to the cue pictures [2]. They were selected from the International Affective Picture System [26] on the basis of low arousal and neutral affect. All pictures were resized to the same dimensions, 6.35 cm x 6.35 cm, using Microsoft Paint and were presented on a gray background.

Procedure

Both psychological and psychophysiological assessments were performed on two different days. The first day, participants were informed about the procedures. Using a semi-structured interview, a psychiatrist assessed the presence of psychiatric comorbidity and family history of alcoholism. Finally, the clinical evaluation using the three scales described above was carried out by an expert. On the second day, participants were assessed with the VPT. Participants were seated in a comfortable chair in front of a high-resolution monitor. Subjects were informed that they would watch a series of slides and they were asked to read the instructions on the screen. After reading the instructions, they were told that their reaction time (RT) was going to be measured and were instructed to look at a fixation cross when it appeared on the screen. A pair of pictures (“cue” and “neutral” images) appeared after the cross: one to the right and one to the left of the screen. This was followed by a dot to the left or right of the screen in the position of one of the pictures. Participants were instructed to press the space bar as quickly as possible to indicate the position (left or right) where the dot had appeared. They were given 6 practice trials [27]. Regarding the measures that we used from the VPT in order to study the attentional bias was: the alcohol-congruent and the alcohol- incongruent measures, and the attentional bias measure. Alcohol-congruent meant that both the image and the dot appeared on the same position (left-left or right-right), whereas alcohol-incongruent was used when the image and the dot appeared on the opposite position (left-right or right-left). Finally, attentional bias was obtained from the substraction of alcohol-congruent minus alcohol-incongruent. Higher negative values involve greater attentional bias to alcohol-related cues.

Statistical analysis

The SPSS statistical package version 15 was used for the statistical analysis. Continuous sociodemographic variables were evaluated using one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) and categorical variables using Chi-square test.

For the psychophysiological analysis, we used a 2-way ANOVA repeated measures design, incorporating groupXcondition, with condition as within-subject factor (alcohol- congruent, alcohol-incongruent), and group (AD, CD and controls) as betweensubject factor. Significant main and interaction effects were further analyzed by post-hoc comparisons with Bonferroni adjusted alpha level. Regarding the differences within the CD subgroup, another 2-way ANOVA repeated measures design was performed, with condition as within-subject factor (alcoholcongruent, alcohol-incongruent), and group (CD-AC and CD-NAC) as between-subject factor. Finally, and in order to study attentional bias differences between the three groups we used a one-way ANOVA, with attentional bias measure as the within-subject factor and group (AD, CD and controls) as the betweensubject factor. For specifically examine alcohol attentional bias in CD, we divided this subgroup in terms of history of alcohol abuse

RESULTS

As shown in Table 1, we did not find statistically significant differences between groups in the sociodemographic variables, as the three groups were paired by age and gender, whereas in clinical variables, we did found statistical differences for the Barrat and the Hamilton scales.

Table 1: Sociodemographic and clinical variables [mean (standard deviation)].

  AD (n=30) CD (n=30) Controls P
Age 47.20 ± 7.40 41.80 ± 6.91 43.26 ± 10.11 Ns
Gender        
Male / Female 26 / 9 26 / 4 23 / 12 Ns
Age of onset of alcohol use (years) 15.20 (3.71) 15.93 (2.90) 23 / 12 Ns
Age of onset of alcohol 28.23 (10.80) -- --  
dependence (years)        
Drinking days (previous 27.30 (2.13) 19.21(2.15) 16.23 (6.90) <0.05
month)        
Standard drinks per drinking 20.01 (9.20) 20.01 (9.20) 2.11 (1.34) <0.01
day        
BARRAT 77.17 (11.97) 63.07 (13.89) 38.00 (13.38) <0.001
Hamilton 10.80 (6.61) 8.07 (5.96) 6 (1.98) <0.001

Abbreviations: AD: Patients Diagnosed with Alcohol Dependence; CD: Patients Diagnosed with Cocaine Dependence

Regarding the psychophysiological measures, we found some statistical differences. Firstly, there was a significant conditionXgroup interaction, F (2,97)=5.367, p=0.05. We obtained that in the AD significant statistical differences appeared between the mean reaction times in the alcohol-congruent condition compared to the alcohol-incrongruent (p=0.040), with lower reaction times for the congruent condition. However, within the control group, the reaction times for the incongruent condition were statistically lower than the congruent (p=0.013) (see figure 1 for the mean reaction times in the two conditions within the three different groups).

Mean reaction times for alcohol-congruent and alcohol-incongruent conditions in AD patients, CD patients and controls.

Figure 1: Mean reaction times for alcohol-congruent and alcohol-incongruent conditions in AD patients, CD patients and controls.

Within the CD group, we obtained a significant condition X group interaction, F (1,58)=7.303, p=0.03. CD with alcohol use patients had greater attentional bias to the congruent stimuli than the CD without alcohol use subgroup (p=0.045).

Finally, and regarding the attentional bias differences between groups, we also obtained statistical significant differences, F (2,97)=5.399, p=0.05. Bonferroni post-hoc test showed that AD had higher attentional bias for alcohol stimuli than controls (MD=25.404, p=0.005). Despite we did not find in this study significant differences within CD patients for the attentional bias measure, this group did also obtained negative values, which indicated attentional bias to alcohol, whereas in controls the vaules were positive (AD<CD<Controls) (see table 2 for the means of attetional bias in the three groups).

Table 2: Comparisons between AD, CD and controls on attentional bias congruent.

AD   CD Control group Mean
(SE)   Mean (SE) Mean (SE)
Attentional bias 11.48 (6.54) -2.05 (5.24) 13.92 (4.91)* (minus incongruent)

*Bonferroni adjustment p <0.05

AD<CD<Controls.Higher values means greater attentional bias to alcohol-related cues.

DISCUSSION

To the best of our knowledge this is the first study regarding attentional bias to alcohol-related cues in patients with cocaine dependence. The most relevant result of our study was the fact that CD subjects showed greater attentional bias to alcohol stimuli than controls. Our data are in accordance with other authors that also reported greater attentional bias in alcohol dependent patients.

Mean reaction times for attentional bias measure (congruent minus incongruent) in AD patients, CD patients and controls.

Figure 2: Mean reaction times for attentional bias measure (congruent minus incongruent) in AD patients, CD patients and controls.

Our results supported our hypothesis concerning the potential greater attentional bias to alcohol-related stimuli in CD patients. Some theoretical models emphasize that once subjects are addicted to any drug, the attentional bias to drugs is already established, and it may theoretically contribute to increased motivation to consume any drug in the future [1]. Furthermore, alcohol priming doses can also increase attentional biases for cues associated with other drugs in drug abusers. For example, in regular smokers alcohol administration (0.4 g/kg) increased attentional bias for tobacco cues and craving for cigarettes [28]. Therefore, alcohol may increase the incentive- motivational properties of a variety of abused substances. This phenomenon of cross- sensitization suggests that dopaminergic sensitization produced by repeated use of one drug (e.g., cocaine) could render the mesolimbic dopamine system hypersensitive to other drugs and then cues associated with any drug [29]. Thus, regular use of cocaine may produce dopaminergic sensitization that renders the individual hypersensitive to alcohol’s dopaminergic effects. Nevertheless, most of the studies have focused on the attentional bias to cocaine-related stimuli in cocaine abuse patients in terms of the influence that alcohol abuse may exert on it [17], but to the best of our knowledge there are no studies focused on the effects that alcohol abuse has on the attentional bias to alcohol-cues in cocaine abusers. Therefore, these current findings may suggest that CD patients might show an attentional bias to other drugs (alcohol in this case) that have been used with cocaine through the addictive course. When we considered alcohol consumption within this CD group, we did find differences in attentional bias to alcohol-stimuli. These data may explain why alcohol use is highly widespread in cocaine abusers [14], and thus this should be an issue of great relevance when studying attentional bias in these patients.

As expected, our results found that attentional bias is impaired in AD patients, with greater attentional bias to alcohol-stimuli. Schema-based theories of addiction suggest that drug using behaviours tend to be automatic, difficult to impede in the presence of triggering stimuli and may occur in the absence of awareness [30], and incentive theories of addiction propose that stimuli associated with drug taking become particularly salient [3]. Both of these theories highlight the ability of drugrelated stimuli to capture attention, and support the idea that attentional biases play an important part in drug dependence [9]. Townshend et al found that stimuli associated with alcohol tend to acquire incentive salience as related theories suggest [31] and capture the attention of the individual who uses the drug [9]. Furthermore, these authors also showed that this attentional bias to alcohol-related cues is still found in non- dependent social drinkers, which suggests that cue-reactivity is sensitive to non- dependent individuals and that cue-reactivity may develop through a history of alcohol use, from first experimentation to abuse and possible dependence [30].

CONCLUSION

This is the first study focused on examining the attentional bias to alcohol-related stimuli in AD patients and in CD patients. Our results indicated that although in AD group there was greater attentional bias to alcohol stimuli, a similar effect occurred in CD subjects. Consequently, we concluded that although attentional bias to alcohol- related cues was clearly found in AD patients, this might also appear in subjects diagnosed with cocaine dependence, as we observed in our sample of research.

Nevertheless, this study is at least subject to some limitations. Firstly, there was higher incidence of male subjects within the three groups, and the gender variable may be influencing the results. However, this difference between genders is representative of the prevalence in substance abuse that is commonly found in the clinical population. Secondly, patients with AD showed higher score in the Hamilton scale and the Barrat scale than CD patients and controls. Therefore, this should be a variable to consider for future studies. Finally, within the CD patients, there was a high prevalence of alcohol abuse. However, this is the representative incidence that occurs in the general population of cocaine abusers, and thus this should be considered when studying attentional bias in these patients.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

The authors want to thank the participants of the study.

REFERENCES

1. Field M, Cox WM. Attentional bias in addictive behaviors: a review of its development, causes, and consequences. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2008; 97: 1-20.

2. Field M, Munafò MR, Franken IH. A meta-analytic investigation of the relationship between attentional bias and subjective craving in substance abuse. Psychol Bull. 2009; 135: 589-607.

3. Robinson TE, Berridge KC. The neural basis of drug craving: an incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 1993; 18: 247-291.

4. Robinson TE, Berridge KC. Addiction. Annu Rev Psychol. 2003; 54: 25- 53.

5. Bindra D. A motivational view of learning, performance, and behavior modification. Psychol Rev. 1974; 81: 199-213.

6. Mackintosh NJ. Blocking of conditioned suppression: role of the first compound trial. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 1975; 1: 335-345.

7. Pearce JM, Bouton ME. Theories of associative learning in animals. Annu Rev Psychol. 2001; 52: 111-139.

8. Hester R, Dixon V, Garavan H. A consistent attentional bias for drug-related material in active cocaine users across word and picture versions of the emotional Stroop task. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2006; 81: 251-257.

9. Townshend JM, Duka T. Attentional bias associated with alcohol cues: differences between heavy and occasional social drinkers. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2001; 157: 67-74.

10. Mogg K, Bradley BP, Field M, De Houwer J. Eye movements to smoking-related pictures in smokers: relationship between attentional biases and implicit and explicit measures of stimulus valence. Addiction. 2003; 98: 825-836.

11. Duka T, Townshend JM. The priming effect of alcohol pre-load on attentional bias to alcohol-related stimuli. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2004; 176: 353-361.

12. Townshend JM, Duka T. Avoidance of alcohol-related stimuli in alcohol-dependent inpatients. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2007; 31: 1349- 1357.

13. Nikolaou K, Critchley H, Duka T. Alcohol affects neuronal substrates of response inhibition but not of perceptual processing of stimuli signalling a stop response. PLoS One. 2013; 8: e76649.

14. Gossop M, Manning V, Ridge G. Concurrent use of alcohol and cocaine: differences in patterns of use and problems among users of crack cocaine and cocaine powder. Alcohol Alcohol. 2006; 41: 121-125.

15. Pennings EJ, Leccese AP, Wolff FA. Effects of concurrent use of alcohol and cocaine. Addiction. 2002; 97: 773-783.

16. Franken IH, Hendriks VM, Stam CJ, Van den Brink W. A role for dopamine in the processing of drug cues in heroin dependent patients. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2004; 14: 503-508.

17. Montgomery C, Field M, Atkinson AM, Cole JC, Goudie AJ, Sumnall HR. Effects of alcohol preload on attentional bias towards cocaine-related cues. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2010; 210: 365-375.

18. Rubio G, Manzanares J, Jiménez M, Rodríguez-Jiménez R, Martínez I, Iribarren MM, et al. Use of cocaine by heavy drinkers increases vulnerability to developing alcohol dependence: a 4-year follow-up study. J Clin Psychiatry. 2008; 69: 563-570.

19. APA, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. 2000.

20. Torrens M, Serrano D, Astals M, Pérez-Domínguez G, Martín-Santos R. Diagnosing comorbid psychiatric disorders in substance abusers: validity of the Spanish versions of the Research Interview for Substance and Mental Disorders and the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Am J Psychiatry. 2004; 161: 1231-1237.

21. Rubio G, Urosa B, Santo-Domingo J. Validación de la escala de la intensidad de la dependencia al alcohol. Psiquiatría Biológica. 1998; 5: 44-47.

22. Hamilton M. A rating scale for depression. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1960; 23: 56-62.

23. Patton JH, Stanford MS, Barratt ES. Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. J Clin Psychol. 1995; 51: 768-774.

24. Lubman DI, Peters LA, Mogg K, Bradley BP, Deakin JF. Attentional bias for drug cues in opiate dependence. Psychol Med. 2000; 30: 169-175.

25. Bradley MM, Lang PJ. Measuring emotion: the Self-Assessment Manikin and the Semantic Differential. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 1994; 25: 49-59.

26. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JW. Structured clinical interview for DSM–IV–TR axis I disorders, research version, non-patient ed. (SCID-I/NP). New York: Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute. 2002.

27. Lang PJ, Öhman A, Vaitl D. The international affective picture system [Photographic slides]. Gainesville, FL: University of Florida, the Center for Research in Psychophysiology. 1988.

28. Field M, Eastwood B. Experimental manipulation of attentional bias increases the motivation to drink alcohol. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2005; 183: 350-357.

29. Biala G, Budzynska B. Calcium-dependent mechanisms of the reinstatement of nicotine-conditioned place preference by drug priming in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2008; 89: 116-125.

30. Tiffany ST. A cognitive model of drug urges and drug-use behavior: role of automatic and nonautomatic processes. Psychol Rev. 1990; 97: 147-168.

31. Stewart J, de Wit H, Eikelboom R. Role of unconditioned and conditioned drug effects in the self-administration of opiates and stimulants. Psychol Rev. 1984; 91: 251-268.

Received : 20 Dec 2013
Accepted : 07 Feb 2014
Published : 18 Feb 2014
Journals
Annals of Otolaryngology and Rhinology
ISSN : 2379-948X
Launched : 2014
JSM Schizophrenia
Launched : 2016
Journal of Nausea
Launched : 2020
JSM Internal Medicine
Launched : 2016
JSM Hepatitis
Launched : 2016
JSM Oro Facial Surgeries
ISSN : 2578-3211
Launched : 2016
Journal of Human Nutrition and Food Science
ISSN : 2333-6706
Launched : 2013
JSM Regenerative Medicine and Bioengineering
ISSN : 2379-0490
Launched : 2013
JSM Spine
ISSN : 2578-3181
Launched : 2016
Archives of Palliative Care
ISSN : 2573-1165
Launched : 2016
JSM Nutritional Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3203
Launched : 2017
Annals of Neurodegenerative Disorders
ISSN : 2476-2032
Launched : 2016
Journal of Fever
ISSN : 2641-7782
Launched : 2017
JSM Bone Marrow Research
ISSN : 2578-3351
Launched : 2016
JSM Mathematics and Statistics
ISSN : 2578-3173
Launched : 2014
Journal of Autoimmunity and Research
ISSN : 2573-1173
Launched : 2014
JSM Arthritis
ISSN : 2475-9155
Launched : 2016
JSM Head and Neck Cancer-Cases and Reviews
ISSN : 2573-1610
Launched : 2016
JSM General Surgery Cases and Images
ISSN : 2573-1564
Launched : 2016
JSM Anatomy and Physiology
ISSN : 2573-1262
Launched : 2016
JSM Dental Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1548
Launched : 2016
Annals of Emergency Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1017
Launched : 2016
Annals of Mens Health and Wellness
ISSN : 2641-7707
Launched : 2017
Journal of Preventive Medicine and Health Care
ISSN : 2576-0084
Launched : 2018
Journal of Chronic Diseases and Management
ISSN : 2573-1300
Launched : 2016
Annals of Vaccines and Immunization
ISSN : 2378-9379
Launched : 2014
JSM Heart Surgery Cases and Images
ISSN : 2578-3157
Launched : 2016
Annals of Reproductive Medicine and Treatment
ISSN : 2573-1092
Launched : 2016
JSM Brain Science
ISSN : 2573-1289
Launched : 2016
JSM Biomarkers
ISSN : 2578-3815
Launched : 2014
JSM Biology
ISSN : 2475-9392
Launched : 2016
Archives of Stem Cell and Research
ISSN : 2578-3580
Launched : 2014
Annals of Clinical and Medical Microbiology
ISSN : 2578-3629
Launched : 2014
JSM Pediatric Surgery
ISSN : 2578-3149
Launched : 2017
Journal of Memory Disorder and Rehabilitation
ISSN : 2578-319X
Launched : 2016
JSM Tropical Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2578-3165
Launched : 2016
JSM Head and Face Medicine
ISSN : 2578-3793
Launched : 2016
JSM Cardiothoracic Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1297
Launched : 2016
JSM Bone and Joint Diseases
ISSN : 2578-3351
Launched : 2017
JSM Bioavailability and Bioequivalence
ISSN : 2641-7812
Launched : 2017
JSM Atherosclerosis
ISSN : 2573-1270
Launched : 2016
Journal of Genitourinary Disorders
ISSN : 2641-7790
Launched : 2017
Journal of Fractures and Sprains
ISSN : 2578-3831
Launched : 2016
Journal of Autism and Epilepsy
ISSN : 2641-7774
Launched : 2016
Annals of Marine Biology and Research
ISSN : 2573-105X
Launched : 2014
JSM Health Education & Primary Health Care
ISSN : 2578-3777
Launched : 2016
JSM Communication Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3807
Launched : 2016
Annals of Musculoskeletal Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3599
Launched : 2016
Annals of Virology and Research
ISSN : 2573-1122
Launched : 2014
JSM Renal Medicine
ISSN : 2573-1637
Launched : 2016
Journal of Muscle Health
ISSN : 2578-3823
Launched : 2016
JSM Genetics and Genomics
ISSN : 2334-1823
Launched : 2013
JSM Anxiety and Depression
ISSN : 2475-9139
Launched : 2016
Clinical Journal of Heart Diseases
ISSN : 2641-7766
Launched : 2016
Annals of Medicinal Chemistry and Research
ISSN : 2378-9336
Launched : 2014
JSM Pain and Management
ISSN : 2578-3378
Launched : 2016
JSM Women's Health
ISSN : 2578-3696
Launched : 2016
Clinical Research in HIV or AIDS
ISSN : 2374-0094
Launched : 2013
Journal of Endocrinology, Diabetes and Obesity
ISSN : 2333-6692
Launched : 2013
JSM Neurosurgery and Spine
ISSN : 2373-9479
Launched : 2013
Journal of Liver and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2379-0830
Launched : 2014
Journal of Drug Design and Research
ISSN : 2379-089X
Launched : 2014
JSM Clinical Oncology and Research
ISSN : 2373-938X
Launched : 2013
JSM Bioinformatics, Genomics and Proteomics
ISSN : 2576-1102
Launched : 2014
JSM Chemistry
ISSN : 2334-1831
Launched : 2013
Journal of Trauma and Care
ISSN : 2573-1246
Launched : 2014
JSM Surgical Oncology and Research
ISSN : 2578-3688
Launched : 2016
Annals of Food Processing and Preservation
ISSN : 2573-1033
Launched : 2016
Journal of Radiology and Radiation Therapy
ISSN : 2333-7095
Launched : 2013
JSM Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation
ISSN : 2578-3572
Launched : 2016
Annals of Clinical Pathology
ISSN : 2373-9282
Launched : 2013
Annals of Cardiovascular Diseases
ISSN : 2641-7731
Launched : 2016
Journal of Behavior
ISSN : 2576-0076
Launched : 2016
Annals of Clinical and Experimental Metabolism
ISSN : 2572-2492
Launched : 2016
Clinical Research in Infectious Diseases
ISSN : 2379-0636
Launched : 2013
JSM Microbiology
ISSN : 2333-6455
Launched : 2013
Journal of Urology and Research
ISSN : 2379-951X
Launched : 2014
Journal of Family Medicine and Community Health
ISSN : 2379-0547
Launched : 2013
Annals of Pregnancy and Care
ISSN : 2578-336X
Launched : 2017
JSM Cell and Developmental Biology
ISSN : 2379-061X
Launched : 2013
Annals of Aquaculture and Research
ISSN : 2379-0881
Launched : 2014
Clinical Research in Pulmonology
ISSN : 2333-6625
Launched : 2013
Journal of Immunology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2333-6714
Launched : 2013
Annals of Forensic Research and Analysis
ISSN : 2378-9476
Launched : 2014
JSM Biochemistry and Molecular Biology
ISSN : 2333-7109
Launched : 2013
Annals of Breast Cancer Research
ISSN : 2641-7685
Launched : 2016
Annals of Gerontology and Geriatric Research
ISSN : 2378-9409
Launched : 2014
Journal of Sleep Medicine and Disorders
ISSN : 2379-0822
Launched : 2014
JSM Burns and Trauma
ISSN : 2475-9406
Launched : 2016
Chemical Engineering and Process Techniques
ISSN : 2333-6633
Launched : 2013
Annals of Clinical Cytology and Pathology
ISSN : 2475-9430
Launched : 2014
JSM Allergy and Asthma
ISSN : 2573-1254
Launched : 2016
Journal of Neurological Disorders and Stroke
ISSN : 2334-2307
Launched : 2013
Annals of Sports Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2379-0571
Launched : 2014
JSM Sexual Medicine
ISSN : 2578-3718
Launched : 2016
Annals of Vascular Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2378-9344
Launched : 2014
JSM Biotechnology and Biomedical Engineering
ISSN : 2333-7117
Launched : 2013
Journal of Hematology and Transfusion
ISSN : 2333-6684
Launched : 2013
JSM Environmental Science and Ecology
ISSN : 2333-7141
Launched : 2013
Journal of Cardiology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2333-6676
Launched : 2013
JSM Nanotechnology and Nanomedicine
ISSN : 2334-1815
Launched : 2013
Journal of Ear, Nose and Throat Disorders
ISSN : 2475-9473
Launched : 2016
JSM Ophthalmology
ISSN : 2333-6447
Launched : 2013
Journal of Pharmacology and Clinical Toxicology
ISSN : 2333-7079
Launched : 2013
Annals of Psychiatry and Mental Health
ISSN : 2374-0124
Launched : 2013
Medical Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology
ISSN : 2333-6439
Launched : 2013
Annals of Pediatrics and Child Health
ISSN : 2373-9312
Launched : 2013
JSM Clinical Pharmaceutics
ISSN : 2379-9498
Launched : 2014
JSM Foot and Ankle
ISSN : 2475-9112
Launched : 2016
JSM Alzheimer's Disease and Related Dementia
ISSN : 2378-9565
Launched : 2014
Journal of Addiction Medicine and Therapy
ISSN : 2333-665X
Launched : 2013
Journal of Veterinary Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2378-931X
Launched : 2013
Annals of Public Health and Research
ISSN : 2378-9328
Launched : 2014
Annals of Orthopedics and Rheumatology
ISSN : 2373-9290
Launched : 2013
Journal of Clinical Nephrology and Research
ISSN : 2379-0652
Launched : 2014
Annals of Community Medicine and Practice
ISSN : 2475-9465
Launched : 2014
Annals of Biometrics and Biostatistics
ISSN : 2374-0116
Launched : 2013
JSM Clinical Case Reports
ISSN : 2373-9819
Launched : 2013
Journal of Cancer Biology and Research
ISSN : 2373-9436
Launched : 2013
Journal of Surgery and Transplantation Science
ISSN : 2379-0911
Launched : 2013
Journal of Dermatology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2373-9371
Launched : 2013
JSM Gastroenterology and Hepatology
ISSN : 2373-9487
Launched : 2013
Annals of Nursing and Practice
ISSN : 2379-9501
Launched : 2014
JSM Dentistry
ISSN : 2333-7133
Launched : 2013
Author Information X