Loading

JSM Ophthalmology

Apparent Motion for UltraSmall offsets as a Hyperacuity Phenomenon

Research Article | Open Access | Volume 5 | Issue 3

  • 1. Department of Psychology, Lomonosov Moscow State University, Russia
+ Show More - Show Less
Corresponding Authors
Garusev Alexander, Faculty of Psychology, Lomonosov Moscow State University, Mokhovaya st. 11/9 Moscow 125009, Russia, Tel: 7-495-629-75-54;
Keywords

Visual acuity , Two-point resolution , Hyperacuity , Apparent motion

ABBREVIATIONS

MAR? Minimum Angle of Resolution

INTRODUCTION

The term visual acuity usually implies the ability of a subject to discriminate very small objects at a considerable distance. The commonly accepted approach to determine visual acuity is based on Hermann Helmholtz’s metaphor: “the eye is like a camera”. An optical system forms an image in the retinal plane that is converted into electrical signals by an array of photoreceptors. These signals are transformed by retinal neurons and then transmitted through the optic nerve to the brain for further analysis. Each of these processes affects the visual acuity.

It is customary to assume that the optical system of the eye essentially sets the upper limit of visual acuity. The diameter and spacing of the photoreceptors are in good agreement with the size of the point source diffraction image. The basic concept here is the resolving power of an optical system, characterizing its ability to produce separate images of two closely spaced light sources. To assess the quality of images on the retina, optical methods are used to identify an ophthalmic model of the eye.

The angular size of test stimuli which the observer can still distinguish with a given probability is usually taken as a measure of visual acuity. A wide variety of assorted stimuli is used in these experiments: points, lines, grids, various figures, letters, etc. For this purpose, appropriate psychophysical procedures have been developed

Suppose we know how the optical system of the eye form images of a set of simple stimuli. Then, using transfer function linearity, it is always possible to calculate the image of any stimulus that can be expressed as their weighted sum. Thus, if visual acuity was determined only by eye optics, then using the ophthalmological model, one could calculate the sensitivity of the visual system to any stimulation. However, in a number of cases, the results of psychophysical experiments are poorly consistent with predictions based on the ophthalmic model. This refers to a lot of experimental facts known as “hyperacuity” [1]. The thresholds of detection or discrimination for certain stimuli are very low. This is true for Vernier visual acuity, abnormally low stereovision thresholds, apparent motion caused by closely spaced light sources, and the like. In such experiments, the subjects distinguish small details obviously better than it follows from the ophthalmic model.

It seems likely that the hyperacuity can be explained by the cooperative effects [2]. Extended stimuli activate more photoreceptors, whose signals can be processed together. Thus, the effects of accumulation and signal processing in the visual system allow us to perceive much smaller details of the stimulus than one would expect, applying the Rayleigh criterion for imaging on the retina. Despite the huge accumulated array of experimental data, at the moment there is no general psychophysical theory, which allows us to quantitatively predict detection and discrimination thresholds for arbitrary stimuli.

One also may speculate that oculomotor activity plays a large role in the operation of these mechanisms. At the same time, it is not completely clear how the eyes micro-movements are associated with an increase of the resolution in vision. Currently, this issue is being actively explored [3,4].

Thus, an experimental study is now required that could clarify the contribution of each component to the visual acuity:

- Optical system of the eye

- Effects of light field conversion in retina volume and light absorption.

- Eyes micro-movements

- Effects associated with processing signals in the retina and the visual fields of the brain

The solution of this problem is greatly simplified if we choose a stimulation that would allow us to separately investigate the influence of each of these factors. In particular, in order to exclude the cooperative effect discussed above, it is desirable to abandon the use of extended stimuli and use point stimulation. This allows minimizing the number of simultaneously activated photoreceptors. To eliminate the effect of eye movement, stimuli should be presented for a short time. Moreover, due to Bloch’s law for temporal summation, the shorter is the presentation time, the higher brightness of the point stimulus is required for the confident detection.

Broadly speaking, experimental study of the detection of luminous points is challenging task. From Ricco’s law, the detection threshold is proportional to the product of brightness and the area of the stimulus. This means that the smaller the stimulus size, the greater the brightness is required for the stimulus to be detected with confidence. To build up the smallsized point light stimulus of the angular size comparable with the diameter of a single photoreceptor at a distance of 2-6 meters from the screen (that is typical for laboratory experiments), only one pixel should be used. The brightness of a standard monitor is insufficient for this.

Probably for this reason, mainly two types of stimulation for visual acuity investigation can be found in the literature: point light stimuli with small angular size and relatively long presentation time (more than 100 ms), or area stimuli (more than 2 arcmin). At the same time, there are practically no experiments in which point stimuli whose angular size is smaller than the size of a single photoreceptor would be presented for a short time.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Therefore, despite all the comforts, we must abandon the use of monitors in such experiments. We developed a visual testing instrument (Figure 1) that meets the requirements: extremely small angular dimensions, short-term presentation, and, consequently, high brightness. As sources of light, ultra-bright light-emitting diodes LED1 and LED2 are used. Light beams are formed by the diaphragms D1 and D2 and are merged at the beam splitter cube BS. LED2 is mounted on a movable carriage and is driven by a stepper motor with a corkscrew shaft. In this way, the optical separation between light sources can be adjusted. With an observation distance of about three meters, the angular diameter of the light source is 7 arc sec (Figure 1b). The angular distance between the light sources can be set with the step of 1 arc sec. The luminance of the light sources is approximately 4000 cd/m2 . All device parameters are controlled by microprocessor.

A similar design was used by [5], but instead of LEDs, laser light sources were employed. This can be the cause of the appearance of speckles – random patterns formed by the mutual interference of coherent waves, scattered on the optical elements of the eye. Speckles can mask the signal and affect the subject’s detection of the stimuli. For this reason, we have refused to use coherent light sources.

Three subjects (A.E., A.K., E.S.) participated in the experiments. The distance from the visual testing instrument to the subject’s eyes was 287 cm. Under these conditions, the point sources of light had angular diameter of 7.2 arc sec. To minimize the effect of eye movements, the exposure time of LED flashes was chosen equal to 12 msec. The angular separation between light sources varied randomly with 10 arc.sec step. The experiments were carried out in a laboratory room with standard illumination with fluorescent lamps of 200 lx.

We started with a single presentation of stimulus in each trial, but it was found that in the experiments described below it was difficult for subjects to make persistent judgments. To ensure that all experiments were conducted under the same conditions, one test always included 5 stimulus repetitions with an interval of 750 ms.

The measurements were carried out by the method of constants.

Experiment 1

Psychophysical measurement of the resolving power of the eye with two-point discrimination. An estimation of the threshold for distinguishing two points was made by simultaneous presentation of pairs of light points, the distance between which varied.

Instruction: subjects were asked whether they saw 1 or 2 points.

The decision-making data are fitted by the logistic curves that are completely determined by the threshold values, where the probability of correct response is 50%, and the scale parameters.

Psychometric curves obtained for three subjects are presented in Figure 2.

The parameters of the fitted logistics for all experiments are tabulated in the Table 1.

The results correspond to the size of the photoreceptors and the diffraction limit of the optics of the eye and compare well with those obtained in other investigations. Let’s give some examples. [5] The minimum angle of resolution (MAR) was determined to be 1.58 to 2.74 arc.min. Time of presentation is not specified in the paper.

[6] The thresholds for the two-point resolution task were around 48 arc sec. Presentation time was 100 ms, angular size of the point source - 12 arc sec.

[7] MAR was about 105 arc sec., the angular size of the source (according to our calculations) is approximately 10 arc sec.

Since in our experiments the presentation time was 12 ms, it can be assumed that the effect of eye micro-movements, according to the data given in [8], can be neglected.

If the light points are not presented simultaneously, then an apparent motion is observed [9,10] first discovered experimentally that apparent motion is observed, even two light points do not differ with simultaneous presentation. The experimental setup of Exner formed two sparks at some distance from each other with a short time interval. Two sparks appeared to the observer as one moving spark. Exner argued that the apparent motion is a separate quality of perception.

There are several papers in which the apparent motion was examined using closely spaced light points, but only for peripheral vision. It has been repeatedly noted that the apparent motion is observed when the points do not differ [11-13]. But in these studies no direct measurements of the two-point resolution were carried out. So, we measured the detection thresholds of apparent motion under the same conditions as in the Experiment 1, where direct measurements of two-point resolution had being conducted.

Experiment 2. The detection thresholds of apparent motion were measured. Subjects were presented with pairs of flashing points. Order of the flashes, the point offsets, and the turn-on delays (0, 20, 40 ms) were varied. Instruction: the subject had to press one of the three buttons, depending on whether he observed motion to the left, to the right, or did not observe the motion at all. With a single presentation of two flashing points, the subject often observed confidently the movement, but could not determine its direction. To make the results more sustainable, the subject made a decision after 5 consecutive presentations in each trial

The results of each experiment are presented as two psychometric curves. The one on the right describes the dependence of the probability of the subject’s answer “there is a movement to the right” depending on the turn-on delay, provided that the left point flashes before the right one. The left curve corresponds to the probability of the answer “there is a movement to the left”, provided that the right point flashes before the left point.

Psychometric functions for all subjects fitted with logistics are shown in Figure 3a and 3b respectively for 20 and 40 ms turn-on delays.

 

Table 1: The parameters of the fitted logistics for all experiments have been summarized in the Table 1.

subject A.E. A.K. E.S.
two-point discrimination
threshold 101 96 77
slope 6 5 5
apparent motion, 20 ms turn-on delay
motion direction
threshold 76 62 62 64 70 63
scaling factor 17 12 14 18 22 15
apparent motion, 40 ms turn-on delay
motion direction
threshold 37 38 34 39 40 37
scaling factor 13 8 8 7 11 10

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

RESULTS AND DISCUSSIONS

1. Based on measurements of two-point resolution and detection thresholds for apparent motion obtained under the same experimental conditions and for the same subjects, it can be asserted that motion detection occurs at a distance between points much less than necessary for distinguishing points. Thus, for foveal vision, the observation of Exner is confirmed.

2. We can assume that the apparent motion with closely spaced stimulus points belongs to the group of hyperacuity phenomena. The fact noted by Exner can be viewed as one of the phenomena of hyperacuity in the sense that the detection threshold of the apparent motion is less than the size of the photoreceptor.

3. To explain mechanisms of hyperacuity it is often attributed with the joint processing of signal from a number of activated photoreceptors. The use of small point stimuli makes it possible to minimize the contribution of this effect, and to study separately the influence of separate mechanisms on visual acuity.

4. It is possible to study separately motion detectors and evaluate the spatiotemporal characteristics of stimuli that can activate them, and also answer the question - where are the motion detectors located.

5. We constructed a testing instrument whose point size is much smaller than the size of the photoreceptor in the fovea. The device allows selectively stimulate small groups of receptors and can be employed for study of the microstructure of the retina using a great variety of psychophysical and psychophysiological methods

6. The short presentation time allows investigating the influence of eye movements on visual acuity. It is possible both to eliminate the influence of the eye movements, reducing the time of stimulation, and to synchronize the stimulation with the movements of the eyes.

7. Visual acuity is the ability to respond to small stimuli. What type of stimulation is chosen to assess visual acuity? This is a matter of convention and the convenience of conducting an experiment. In this sense, local apparent motion is a very good method.

 

 

REFERENCES

1. Westheimer G. Visual hyperacuity. Progress in sensory physiology. 1981; 1: 1-30.

2. Westheimer G. Cooperative neural processes involved in stereoscopic acuity. Exp Brain Res. 1979; 36: 585-597.

3. Ahissar E, Arieli A. Seeing via miniature eye movements: a dynamic hypothesis for vision. Front Comput Neurosci. 2012; 6.

4. Rucci M, Victor JD. The unsteady eye: an information-processing stage, not a bug. Trends Neurosci. 2015; 38: 195-206.

5. Ginis H, Karyotakis N, Plainis S, Pallikaris A. The Effect of Light Intensity on Angular Resolution of the Human Eye as Determined by a Two–Point Discrimination Task. Invest J Ophth Vis Sci. 2006; 47: 1222.

6. Geisler W, Davila KD. Ideal discriminators in spatial vision: two-point stimuli. J Opt Soc Am A. 1985; 2: 1483-1497.

7. Ogle K. On the Resolving Power of the Human Eye. J Opt Soc Am. 1951; 41: 517-520.

8. Ezenman M, Hallett P, Frecker RC. Power spectra for ocular drift and tremor. Vision res. 1985; 25: 1635-1640.

9. Exner S. Uber das Sehen von Bewegungen und die Theorie des zusammengesetzten Auges. Sitzungsberichte der Kaiserlichen Akademie der Wissenschaften. 1876; 72: 156-190.

10. Exner S. Uber optische Bewegungsempfindungen. BIOL. ZBL. 1888; 8: 437–448.

11. Thorson J, Lange G, Biederman-Thorson M. Objective measure of the dynamics of a visual movement illusion. Science. 1969; 164: 1087- 1088.

12. Biederman-Thorson M, Thorson J, Lange GD. Apparent movement due to closely spaced sequentially flashed dots in the human peripheral field of vision. Vision Res. 1971; 11: 889-903.

13. Foster D, Gravano S, Tomoszek A. Acuity for fine-grain motion and for two-dot spacing as a function of retinal eccentricity: Differences in specialization of the central and peripheral retina. Vision Res. 1989; 29: 1017-1031

Citation

Alexander G, Victor D, Alexander C (2017) Apparent Motion for Ultra-Small offsets as a Hyperacuity Phenomenon. JSM Ophthalmol 5(3): 1061.

Abstract

two-LED testing instrument was developed to investigate the mechanisms of hyperacuity. Two point light sources are observed through the beam splitter cube. The luminance of each light point is approximately 4000 cd/m2, the angular size is 7 arc sec at a viewing distance of about three meters. The angular distance between light points is accurate to 1 arc sec, and controlled with a stepper motor. To eliminate the effect of eye movements, the 12 ms flash duration was selected.

Two experiments were performed:

1. Estimation of the discrimination threshold. Two light points were presented simultaneously. Subjects were asked whether they observed 1 or 2 points. The resulting thresholds were in the range 80 -100 arc sec for several subjects.

2. Measurement of the apparent motion thresholds. Two light points were presented with time delays 0, 20, 40 ms the subjects answered whether a movement was occurred, and assessed its direction. The following motion detection thresholds were obtained: for asynchrony 20 ms 60- 80 arc sec, for 40 ms 30-40 arc sec. It is known that for stimuli with different spatial size the apparent motion detection thresholds are less than the two-point resolution. Our findings are in line with these results.

Received : 12 Oct 2017
Accepted : 18 Nov 2017
Published : 18 Nov 2017
Journals
Annals of Otolaryngology and Rhinology
ISSN : 2379-948X
Launched : 2014
JSM Schizophrenia
Launched : 2016
Journal of Nausea
Launched : 2020
JSM Internal Medicine
Launched : 2016
JSM Hepatitis
Launched : 2016
JSM Oro Facial Surgeries
ISSN : 2578-3211
Launched : 2016
Journal of Human Nutrition and Food Science
ISSN : 2333-6706
Launched : 2013
JSM Regenerative Medicine and Bioengineering
ISSN : 2379-0490
Launched : 2013
JSM Spine
ISSN : 2578-3181
Launched : 2016
Archives of Palliative Care
ISSN : 2573-1165
Launched : 2016
JSM Nutritional Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3203
Launched : 2017
Annals of Neurodegenerative Disorders
ISSN : 2476-2032
Launched : 2016
Journal of Fever
ISSN : 2641-7782
Launched : 2017
JSM Bone Marrow Research
ISSN : 2578-3351
Launched : 2016
JSM Mathematics and Statistics
ISSN : 2578-3173
Launched : 2014
Journal of Autoimmunity and Research
ISSN : 2573-1173
Launched : 2014
JSM Arthritis
ISSN : 2475-9155
Launched : 2016
JSM Head and Neck Cancer-Cases and Reviews
ISSN : 2573-1610
Launched : 2016
JSM General Surgery Cases and Images
ISSN : 2573-1564
Launched : 2016
JSM Anatomy and Physiology
ISSN : 2573-1262
Launched : 2016
JSM Dental Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1548
Launched : 2016
Annals of Emergency Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1017
Launched : 2016
Annals of Mens Health and Wellness
ISSN : 2641-7707
Launched : 2017
Journal of Preventive Medicine and Health Care
ISSN : 2576-0084
Launched : 2018
Journal of Chronic Diseases and Management
ISSN : 2573-1300
Launched : 2016
Annals of Vaccines and Immunization
ISSN : 2378-9379
Launched : 2014
JSM Heart Surgery Cases and Images
ISSN : 2578-3157
Launched : 2016
Annals of Reproductive Medicine and Treatment
ISSN : 2573-1092
Launched : 2016
JSM Brain Science
ISSN : 2573-1289
Launched : 2016
JSM Biomarkers
ISSN : 2578-3815
Launched : 2014
JSM Biology
ISSN : 2475-9392
Launched : 2016
Archives of Stem Cell and Research
ISSN : 2578-3580
Launched : 2014
Annals of Clinical and Medical Microbiology
ISSN : 2578-3629
Launched : 2014
JSM Pediatric Surgery
ISSN : 2578-3149
Launched : 2017
Journal of Memory Disorder and Rehabilitation
ISSN : 2578-319X
Launched : 2016
JSM Tropical Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2578-3165
Launched : 2016
JSM Head and Face Medicine
ISSN : 2578-3793
Launched : 2016
JSM Cardiothoracic Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1297
Launched : 2016
JSM Bone and Joint Diseases
ISSN : 2578-3351
Launched : 2017
JSM Bioavailability and Bioequivalence
ISSN : 2641-7812
Launched : 2017
JSM Atherosclerosis
ISSN : 2573-1270
Launched : 2016
Journal of Genitourinary Disorders
ISSN : 2641-7790
Launched : 2017
Journal of Fractures and Sprains
ISSN : 2578-3831
Launched : 2016
Journal of Autism and Epilepsy
ISSN : 2641-7774
Launched : 2016
Annals of Marine Biology and Research
ISSN : 2573-105X
Launched : 2014
JSM Health Education & Primary Health Care
ISSN : 2578-3777
Launched : 2016
JSM Communication Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3807
Launched : 2016
Annals of Musculoskeletal Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3599
Launched : 2016
Annals of Virology and Research
ISSN : 2573-1122
Launched : 2014
JSM Renal Medicine
ISSN : 2573-1637
Launched : 2016
Journal of Muscle Health
ISSN : 2578-3823
Launched : 2016
JSM Genetics and Genomics
ISSN : 2334-1823
Launched : 2013
JSM Anxiety and Depression
ISSN : 2475-9139
Launched : 2016
Clinical Journal of Heart Diseases
ISSN : 2641-7766
Launched : 2016
Annals of Medicinal Chemistry and Research
ISSN : 2378-9336
Launched : 2014
JSM Pain and Management
ISSN : 2578-3378
Launched : 2016
JSM Women's Health
ISSN : 2578-3696
Launched : 2016
Clinical Research in HIV or AIDS
ISSN : 2374-0094
Launched : 2013
Journal of Endocrinology, Diabetes and Obesity
ISSN : 2333-6692
Launched : 2013
Journal of Substance Abuse and Alcoholism
ISSN : 2373-9363
Launched : 2013
JSM Neurosurgery and Spine
ISSN : 2373-9479
Launched : 2013
Journal of Liver and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2379-0830
Launched : 2014
Journal of Drug Design and Research
ISSN : 2379-089X
Launched : 2014
JSM Clinical Oncology and Research
ISSN : 2373-938X
Launched : 2013
JSM Bioinformatics, Genomics and Proteomics
ISSN : 2576-1102
Launched : 2014
JSM Chemistry
ISSN : 2334-1831
Launched : 2013
Journal of Trauma and Care
ISSN : 2573-1246
Launched : 2014
JSM Surgical Oncology and Research
ISSN : 2578-3688
Launched : 2016
Annals of Food Processing and Preservation
ISSN : 2573-1033
Launched : 2016
Journal of Radiology and Radiation Therapy
ISSN : 2333-7095
Launched : 2013
JSM Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation
ISSN : 2578-3572
Launched : 2016
Annals of Clinical Pathology
ISSN : 2373-9282
Launched : 2013
Annals of Cardiovascular Diseases
ISSN : 2641-7731
Launched : 2016
Journal of Behavior
ISSN : 2576-0076
Launched : 2016
Annals of Clinical and Experimental Metabolism
ISSN : 2572-2492
Launched : 2016
Clinical Research in Infectious Diseases
ISSN : 2379-0636
Launched : 2013
JSM Microbiology
ISSN : 2333-6455
Launched : 2013
Journal of Urology and Research
ISSN : 2379-951X
Launched : 2014
Journal of Family Medicine and Community Health
ISSN : 2379-0547
Launched : 2013
Annals of Pregnancy and Care
ISSN : 2578-336X
Launched : 2017
JSM Cell and Developmental Biology
ISSN : 2379-061X
Launched : 2013
Annals of Aquaculture and Research
ISSN : 2379-0881
Launched : 2014
Clinical Research in Pulmonology
ISSN : 2333-6625
Launched : 2013
Journal of Immunology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2333-6714
Launched : 2013
Annals of Forensic Research and Analysis
ISSN : 2378-9476
Launched : 2014
JSM Biochemistry and Molecular Biology
ISSN : 2333-7109
Launched : 2013
Annals of Breast Cancer Research
ISSN : 2641-7685
Launched : 2016
Annals of Gerontology and Geriatric Research
ISSN : 2378-9409
Launched : 2014
Journal of Sleep Medicine and Disorders
ISSN : 2379-0822
Launched : 2014
JSM Burns and Trauma
ISSN : 2475-9406
Launched : 2016
Chemical Engineering and Process Techniques
ISSN : 2333-6633
Launched : 2013
Annals of Clinical Cytology and Pathology
ISSN : 2475-9430
Launched : 2014
JSM Allergy and Asthma
ISSN : 2573-1254
Launched : 2016
Journal of Neurological Disorders and Stroke
ISSN : 2334-2307
Launched : 2013
Annals of Sports Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2379-0571
Launched : 2014
JSM Sexual Medicine
ISSN : 2578-3718
Launched : 2016
Annals of Vascular Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2378-9344
Launched : 2014
JSM Biotechnology and Biomedical Engineering
ISSN : 2333-7117
Launched : 2013
Journal of Hematology and Transfusion
ISSN : 2333-6684
Launched : 2013
JSM Environmental Science and Ecology
ISSN : 2333-7141
Launched : 2013
Journal of Cardiology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2333-6676
Launched : 2013
JSM Nanotechnology and Nanomedicine
ISSN : 2334-1815
Launched : 2013
Journal of Ear, Nose and Throat Disorders
ISSN : 2475-9473
Launched : 2016
Journal of Pharmacology and Clinical Toxicology
ISSN : 2333-7079
Launched : 2013
Annals of Psychiatry and Mental Health
ISSN : 2374-0124
Launched : 2013
Medical Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology
ISSN : 2333-6439
Launched : 2013
Annals of Pediatrics and Child Health
ISSN : 2373-9312
Launched : 2013
JSM Clinical Pharmaceutics
ISSN : 2379-9498
Launched : 2014
JSM Foot and Ankle
ISSN : 2475-9112
Launched : 2016
JSM Alzheimer's Disease and Related Dementia
ISSN : 2378-9565
Launched : 2014
Journal of Addiction Medicine and Therapy
ISSN : 2333-665X
Launched : 2013
Journal of Veterinary Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2378-931X
Launched : 2013
Annals of Public Health and Research
ISSN : 2378-9328
Launched : 2014
Annals of Orthopedics and Rheumatology
ISSN : 2373-9290
Launched : 2013
Journal of Clinical Nephrology and Research
ISSN : 2379-0652
Launched : 2014
Annals of Community Medicine and Practice
ISSN : 2475-9465
Launched : 2014
Annals of Biometrics and Biostatistics
ISSN : 2374-0116
Launched : 2013
JSM Clinical Case Reports
ISSN : 2373-9819
Launched : 2013
Journal of Cancer Biology and Research
ISSN : 2373-9436
Launched : 2013
Journal of Surgery and Transplantation Science
ISSN : 2379-0911
Launched : 2013
Journal of Dermatology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2373-9371
Launched : 2013
JSM Gastroenterology and Hepatology
ISSN : 2373-9487
Launched : 2013
Annals of Nursing and Practice
ISSN : 2379-9501
Launched : 2014
JSM Dentistry
ISSN : 2333-7133
Launched : 2013
Author Information X