Loading

JSM Ophthalmology

Gender Differences in Contrast Thresholds to Biased Stimuli

Review Article | Open Access | Volume 1 | Issue 1

  • 1. Rosenberg School of Optometry, University of the Incarnate Word, USA
  • 2. University of Missouri, St Louis College of Optometry, USA
+ Show More - Show Less
Corresponding Authors
Brian K. Foutch, Rosenberg School of Optometry, University of the Incarnate Word, 9725 Datapoint Drive, San Antonio, TX 78229, Tel: 210-930-8162;
Citation

outch BK, Peck CK (2013) Gender Differences in Contrast Thresholds to Biased Stimuli. JSM Ophthalmol 1: 1007.

Abstract

We present a limited review of the literature on gender differences in visual processing. We then add evidence to that body of literature, reporting the results of an examination of gender differences in response to stimulus conditions favoring magnocellular (MC) and parvocellualr (PC) processing. We measured contrast thresholds and mean reaction times of 24 subjects (15 women, 9 men) to two grating stimuli, one designed to be processed more strongly by MC pathways and the other by PC pathways. There was a significant interaction of gender and stimulus type on contrast thresholds (F = 4.80, p = 0.03) and reaction times (F = 4.13, p = 0.04). Women were more sensitive than men to the PC-biased stimulus (t = 1.94, p = 0.05), but men and women were equally sensitive to the MC-biased stimulus (t = -1.22, p = 0.23). The results of this experiment add to the body of evidence that women may rely more on parvocellular visual processes than men.

Keywords

Gender , Parvocellular , Magnocellular , Contrast sensitivity , Reaction times

INTRODUCTION

In humans and other primates, physiological and behavioral evidence indicates two anatomically and functionally distinct pathways originating in the magnocellular (MC) and parvocellular (PC) retinal ganglion cells [1,2]. Neurons in the MC pathway are more sensitive to object location, movement, low spatial frequency and global analysis of visual scenes. Neurons in the PC pathway are thought to be more involved with object and pattern recognition as well as color (in particular, red-green) opponency [3,4].

Although previous studies of gender effects on visual processing are heterogeneous, as a group they suggest the possibility of sexual dimorphism in parallel visual processing [5]. For example, Kramer et al. found that, in describing large shapes made up of smaller geometrical elements, boys rely more on the overall shape [6]. Girls, however, are more detailed in their descriptions, referring to the smaller shapes to describe the figures. In free drawings of imagined environmental scenes, boys tend to describe more motion and include more mechanical descriptions while girls tend to use more color [7]. In a study designed to determine the effects of gender and age on dynamic visual acuity, Ishigaki and Miyao found that men tended to detect a small gap at faster drifting rates than do women [8]. The results of these studies suggest that men may rely more on MC processing, while women may rely more on PC processing.

The purpose of this work is two-fold. First, we present a limited review of the literature on gender differences in visual processing. Our second purpose is to add evidence to that body of literature, reporting the results of an experiment that examined gender differences in responses to stimulus conditions favoring MC and PC processing. In our experiment, we measured contrast thresholds and mean reaction times to two grating stimuli, one designed to be processed more strongly by MC pathways and the other by PC pathways. We predicted that women would be more sensitive to the PC-biased stimulus.

 

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Subjects were eligible if they were between 18 and 45 years old, reported a complete eye examination within the last twelve months, had best-corrected visual acuity of 20/20 or better in each eye, and had normal color vision when tested with psuedoisochromatic plates. The institutional review board of the University of Missouri – St. Louis approved the experimental protocol, and informed consent was obtained from each participant. Twenty-four adults (fifteen women, nine men) participated in the experiment. Three potential participants self-reported a history of corneal refractive surgery and were excluded from participating based on the reported effects of refractive surgery on the contrast sensitivity function [9,10].

Vision Works 4.0 Contrast Sensitivity Software (Vision Research Graphics, Durham, NH) was used to generate and display stimuli on a 21” RGB analog monitor (FlexScanF750i, Eizo Nanao Technologies Inc., Cypress, CA) located 250 cm from the observer. Monocular contrast thresholds were obtained using a QUEST modified staircase algorithm [11]. Contrast thresholds as well as mean reaction times for all positive (yes) responses were analyzed for main effects of gender by repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA). All analyses were performed using SPSS statistical software (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL).The MC-biased stimulus was a rectangular, one cycle per degree (cpd) achromatic (black and white) grating subtending three (high) by five (wide)

degrees of visual angle. The stimulus also had a surround configuration to limit foveal viewing of the stimulus, and— while the overall stimulus remained stationary—the grating pattern drifted to the left at 30 cycles per second (cps). The PCbiased stimulus was a stationary circular target subtending 1.25 degrees of visual angle. The target was a red on green grating of 20 cpd and did not drift. In addition to the two primary stimuli, four additional stimuli with varied center, full or surround characteristics were randomly interwoven into the stimulus presentation. Some characteristics of these additional stimuli were not as biased toward MC and PC processing. Therefore, only the results from the two primary stimuli are reported here. The onset of each trial was signaled by a short tone, after which a single stimulus was presented. Trials for all six stimuli were interleaved, and participants used a keyboard stroke to indicate whether or not they could detect a grating pattern. Each keystroke was detected and only positive responses were used for reaction time analysis. Contrast thresholds were calculated as the Michelson ratio: (LF - LB)/(LF + LB), where LF and LB represent foreground and background luminance, respectively. For each subject, a single monocular contrast threshold measurement was made for each stimulus using each eye. These monocular measurements were averaged into a single measure for analysis.

 

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION

Contrast thresholds were positively skewed and were therefore log transformed for analysis. Mean reaction times were normally distributed and represent the mean from all positive (yes) response times for each stimulus. Log transformed contrast thresholds and raw mean reaction times were analyzed for main effects of stimulus type and gender by repeated-measures ANOVA. Simple effects of gender for each stimulus were analyzed using independent t-tests.

Contrast thresholds

For graphical comparisons, contrast thresholds with 95% confidence intervals for each stimulus type are shown in Figure 1. As shown in Table 1, contrast thresholds for the MCbiased stimulus were significantly lower than for the PC-biased stimulus (F = 246, p < 0.001). This result is consistent with many previous findings of increased contrast gain from magnocellular pathways (e.g., Purpura et al. [12]). The main effect of gender was not significant (F = 2.43, p = 0.12), but there was a significant interaction of gender and stimulus type (PC-biased vs. MCbiased) on contrast thresholds (F = 4.80, p = 0.03). As shown in Figure 1, women were more sensitive than men to the PC-biased stimulus (t = 1.94, p = 0.05), but men and women were equally sensitive to the MC-biased stimulus (t = -1.22, p = 0.23).

Mean reaction times

Similar effects of gender and stimulus type were found for reaction times, as shown in Table 1 and Figure 2. Both men and women had significantly lower mean reaction times for the MCbiased stimulus than for the PC-biased stimulus (F = 93.0, p < 0.001). This result is consistent with those of previous studies indicating decreased reaction times to larger and more luminous stimuli [13,14]. There was no main effect of gender (F = 0.50, p = 0.48), but there was a significant interaction of gender and stimulus type on reaction times (F = 4.13, p = 0.04). Unlike the results for contrast thresholds, there was no gender difference in reaction times for either the MC or PC-biased stimulus.

In this experiment, we found that women were more sensitive than men to the contrast changes in the small, redgreen, stationary stimulus, which is more likely to be processed strongly by the PC pathway. This result is in agreement with previous studies indicating that women may rely more on PC processing than males [6,15]. Men had lower contrast thresholds than women to the large, achromatic, drifting stimulus, but the difference was not statistically significant for this target. While previous studies [6-8] indicate a male advantage in some MC processes, the results of studies measuring gender differences in contrast thresholds across spatial frequencies are equivocal [16,17]. Therefore, the lack of a significant finding for the MCbiased stimulus is not unexpected. While neither stimulus is absolutely processed by one parallel pathway or the other, it is reasonable to assume that PC processes underlie sensitivity to the small, red-green target. Likewise, processing for the large, drifting stimulus is certainly biased toward the MC pathways.

Mean reaction times were also used as a measure of visual performance as in at least one previous study [18]. The interaction effect of stimulus type and gender on mean reaction times was significant, but there were no significant gender differences in mean reaction times for either the MC- or PC-biased stimulus. Overall, the results of this experiment add to the body of evidence indicating that there are gender differences in parallel visual processing.

Possible mechanisms for gender differences

Approximately fifteen percent of human females are carriers of sex-linked color deficiencies [19]. In theory, these individuals should have normal color vision since the normal color gene on one of their X chromosomes is dominant and produces a normal phenotype. However, heterozygous carriers often exhibit mild color vision deficiencies on clinical testing. Lyon hypothesized that heterozygous carriers are actually hemizygous in some cells due to X-inactivation during early development of the female embryo [20]. That is, one gene (normal or abnormal) is active at any given point during embryonic development of certain tissues. In the eye, this results in a mosaic of normal and abnormal retina. At least one study [21] has suggested that heterozygous carriers have essentially normal trichromatic vision, while others have found reduced luminous efficiency to long wavelength [22,23]. It is possible, then, that the results of this study indicating gender differences in sensitivity to red-green stimuli were influenced by the inclusion of undetected heterozygous carriers in our study population. However, if some of our female subjects are indeed heterozygous carriers for red-green deficiency, evidence indicates that the advantage in red-green contrast sensitivity might belong to men due to deficient red-green discrimination found in heterozygous carriers [24-26]. It is then unlikely that inclusion of heterozygous carriers for color deficiencies would have produced an overall advantage for the women in our study. Estrogen is a steroid hormone that has significant effects on both male and female reproductive systems. Estrogens also regulate tissue functions outside of the reproductive system. Estrogen receptor (ER) proteins mediate estrogen effects, and at least one type, ERβ , has been observed in both male and female ocular structures, including the iris, lacrimal gland, and choroid [27]. ERβ proteins have also been found in the retinas of premenopausal women, but not in men or in postmenopausal women. These findings suggest the possibility of direct estrogenic influences on visual processing in the premenopausal women who served as subjects in this study.

It is also possible that estrogens act through an intermediate mechanism.Estrogens influence dopamine release by augmenting glutamate and inhibiting gamma amino butyric acid (GABA) syntheses [28]. It is generally accepted that GABA-mediated cortical inhibition is important in determining visual responses [29], and there is research attributing some differences in visual processing abilities to sex hormone levels [30-32]. A review by Parlee [33] highlighted evidence for cyclical effects on visual processing, and a later review [34] of this research suggests there is an increased cortical capacity for visual information processing in women during peak estradiol levels of the menstrual cycle.

Only a few studies have investigated the effects of the menstrual cycle on color vision. Finkelstein [35] suggested an increase in the wavelength position of unique green (toward yellow) as well as restricted color visual fields during the low estrogen menstrual phase. Lorenzetti [36] also found constricted red and green color fields with decreased yellow and green sensitivity during the menstrual phase. While others [37,38] have not been able to demonstrate repeatable changes in standard achromatic visual fields across the menstrual cycle, at least one study [39] has suggested decreased sensitivity to shortwavelength stimuli during the relatively low estradiol luteal phase. In a study of cyclical effects on isolated S-, M- and L-cone mechanisms, Eisner et al. [40] found cyclical effects on S-cone mechanisms, but the cyclical effects were more limited for M- and L-cone mechanisms.

In addition to estrogen, progesterone is implicated in visual processing. Progesterone is thought to antagonize estrogens by increasing CNS sensitivity to GABA and decreasing the CNS response to glutamate [41]. It is then possible that cycling estrogen and progesterone or their interaction enhance PCprocessing in women. 

Table 1: Effect of stimulus type and gender on contrast thresholds and mean reaction times

  Contrast Thresholds Contrast Thresholds
Main Effec F p F p
Stimulus Type *246 < 0.001 *93.0 < 0.001
Gender 2.43 0.12 0.50 0.48
Stimulus x Gender *4.80 0.03 *4.13 0.04
Gender Effect a t p t p
MC-biased -1.22 0.23 -1.16 0.25
PC-biased *1.94 0.05 1.66 0.10

a Gender effect = independent t-test of [values for men - values for women] * p ≤ 0.05

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

CONCLUSION

The results reported here add to the body of evidence that women may rely more on PC visual processing than do men. However, PC processing produces a bias for both color-opponency (particularly red-green) and fine, spatial details. Unfortunately, our experiment did not separate these aspects, and further investigation is warranted into whether the female advantage is based on chromatic or spatial aspects of visual processing or perhaps a combination. In addition, it would be of interest to determine whether the effects reported here are cyclical, relying on hormonal changes measured across the menstrual cycle.

 

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

We acknowledge Carl Bassi, PhD, at the University of Missouri – St Louis for his contributions to the construction of the stimuli and the design used in this experiment.

REFERENCES

1. Leventhal AG, Rodieck RW, Dreher B. Retinal ganglion cell classes in the Old World monkey: morphology and central projections. Science. 1981; 213: 1139-1142.

2. Perry VH, Oehler R, Cowey A. Retinal ganglion cells that project to the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus in the macaque monkey. Neuroscience. 1984; 12: 1101-1123.

3. Schiller PH, Malpeli JG. Functional specificity of lateral geniculate nucleus laminae of the rhesus monkey. J Neurophysiol. 1978; 41: 788- 797.

4. DeYoe EA, Van Essen DC. Concurrent processing streams in monkey visual cortex. Trends Neurosci. 1988; 11: 219-226.

5. Alexander GM. An evolutionary perspective of sex-typed toy preferences: pink, blue, and the brain. Arch Sex Behav. 2003; 32: 7-14.

6. Kramer JH, Ellenberg L, Leonard J, Share LJ. Developmental sex differences in global-local perceptual bias. Neuropsychology 1996; 10: 402-407.

7. Iijima M, Arisaka O, Minamoto F, Arai Y. Sex differences in children’s free drawings: a study on girls with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Horm Behav. 2001; 40: 99-104.

8. Ishigaki H, Miyao M. Implications for dynamic visual acuity with changes in aged and sex. Percept Mot Skills. 1994; 78: 363-369.

9. Niesen U, Businger U, Hartmann P, Senn P, Schipper I. Glare sensitivity and visual acuity after excimer laser photorefractive keratectomy for myopia. Br J Ophthalmol. 1997; 81: 136-140.

10. Nio YK, Jansonius NM, Wijdh RH, Beekhuis WH, Worst JG, Norrby S, et al. Effect of methods of myopia correction on visual acuity, contrast sensitivity, and depth of focus. J Cataract Refract Surg. 2003; 29: 2082- 2095.

11. Watson AB, Pelli DG. QUEST: a Bayesian adaptive psychometric method. Percept Psychophys. 1983; 33: 113-120.

12. Purpura K, Kaplan E, Shapley RM. Background light and the contrast gain of primate P and M retinal ganglion cells. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1988; 85: 4534-4537.

13. Plainis S, Murray IJ. Neurophysiological interpretation of human visual reaction times: effect of contrast, spatial frequency and luminance. Neuropsychologia. 2000; 38: 1555-1564.

14. O’Donell BM, Colombo EM. Reaction times to chromaticity, luminance contrast, size, and adaptation luminance changes. Perception 2006; 35: 19.

15. Bimler DL, Kirkland J, Jameson K. Quantifying variations in personal color spaces: are there sex differences in color vision? Color Res Appl. 2004; 29: 128-134.

16. Oen FT, Lim TH, Chung MP. Contrast sensitivity in a large adult population. Ann Acad Med Singapore. 1994; 23: 322-326.

17. Solberg JL, Brown JM. No sex differences in contrast sensitivity and reaction time to spatial frequency. Percept Mot Skills. 2002; 94: 1053- 1055.

18. Rea MS, Ouellette MJ. Visual performance using reaction times. Light Res Tech. 1988; 20: 139-153.

19. Jordan G, Mollon JD. A study of women heterozygous for colour deficiencies. Vision Res. 1993; 33: 1495-1508.

20. Lyon MF. Lyonisation of the x chromosome. Lancet. 1963; 2: 1120- 1121.

21. Miyahara E, Pokorny J, Smith VC, Baron R, Baron E. Color vision in two observers with highly biased LWS/MWS cone ratios. Vision Res. 1998; 38: 601-612.

22. Schmidt I. A sign of manifest heterozygosity in carriers of color deficiency. Am J Optom Arch Am Acad Optom. 1955; 32: 404-408.

23. Harris RW, Cole BL. Diagnosing protan heterozygosity using the Medmont C-100 colour vision test. Clin Exp Optom. 2005; 88: 240- 247. Foutch BK, Peck CK (2013) Gender Differences in Contrast Thresholds to Biased Stimuli. JSM Ophthalmol 1: 1007. Cite this article

24. Krill AE, Schneiderman A. A hue discrimination defect in so-called normal carriers of color vision defects. Invest Ophthalmol. 1964; 3: 445-450.

25. Lang A, Good GW. Color discrimination in heterozygous deutan carriers. Optom Vis Sci. 2001; 78: 584-588.

26. Hood SM, Mollon JD, Purves L, Jordan G. Color discrimination in carriers of color deficiency. Vision Res. 2006; 46: 2894-2900.

27. Ogueta SB, Schwartz SD, Yamashita CK, Farber DB. Estrogen receptor in the human eye: influence of gender and age on gene expression. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1999; 40: 1906-1911.

28. Smith SS. Estrogen administration increases neuronal responses to excitatory amino acids as a long-term effect. Brain Res. 1989; 503: 354-357.

29. Zemon V, Kaplan E, Ratliff F. Biochemistry and Pharmacology. In: Cracco R, Bodis-Wollner I, editors. Evoked Potentials. New York: Liss. 1986: 290.

30. Diamond M, Diamond AL, Mast M. Visual sensitivity and sexual arousal levels during the menstrual cycle. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1972; 155: 170- 176.

31. Ward MM, Stone SC, Sandman CA. Visual perception in women during the menstrual cycle. Physiol Behav. 1978; 20: 239-243.

32. Yilmaz H, Erkin EF, Mavioglu H, Sungurtekin U. Changes in pattern reversal evoked potentials during menstrual cycle. Int Ophthalmol. 1998; 22: 27-30.

33. Parlee MB. Menstrual rhythms in sensory processes: a review of fluctuations in vision, olfaction, audition, taste, and touch. Psychol Bull. 1983; 93: 539-548.

34. Guttridge NM. Changes in ocular and visual variables during the menstrual cycle. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 1994; 14: 38-48.

35. Finkelstein LO. On sensory disorders in diseases, and on changes of the fields of vision in menstruation. Ophth Rev. 1887; 6: 323-326.

36. Lorenzetti F. Contribution to the study of visual field and chromatic sense of women during the menstrual cycle (translated). Clin Ostet 1926; 48: 345-349.

37. Guttridge N. Mood, pain and the menstrual cycle: potential confounding factors in automated perimetry? Ophth Physl Opt. 1996; 16: 355-356.

38. Akar Y, Yucel I, Akar ME, Taskin O, Ozer HO. Menstrual cycle-dependent changes in visual field analysis of healthy women. Ophthalmologica. 2005; 219: 30-35.

39. Yucel I, Akar ME, Dora B, Akar Y, Taskin O, Ozer HO, et al. Effect of the menstrual cycle on standard achromatic and blue-on-yellow visual field analysis of women with migraine. Can J Ophthalmol. 2005; 40: 51-57.

40. Eisner A, Burke SN, Toomey MD. Visual sensitivity across the menstrual cycle. Vis Neurosci. 2004; 21: 513-531.

41. Smith SS, Waterhouse BD, Chapin JK, Woodward DJ. Progesterone alters GABA and glutamate responsiveness: a possible mechanism for its anxiolytic action. Brain Res. 1987; 400: 353-359

Received : 09 Aug 2013
Accepted : 22 Aug 2013
Published : 27 Aug 2013
Journals
Annals of Otolaryngology and Rhinology
ISSN : 2379-948X
Launched : 2014
JSM Schizophrenia
Launched : 2016
Journal of Nausea
Launched : 2020
JSM Internal Medicine
Launched : 2016
JSM Hepatitis
Launched : 2016
JSM Oro Facial Surgeries
ISSN : 2578-3211
Launched : 2016
Journal of Human Nutrition and Food Science
ISSN : 2333-6706
Launched : 2013
JSM Regenerative Medicine and Bioengineering
ISSN : 2379-0490
Launched : 2013
JSM Spine
ISSN : 2578-3181
Launched : 2016
Archives of Palliative Care
ISSN : 2573-1165
Launched : 2016
JSM Nutritional Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3203
Launched : 2017
Annals of Neurodegenerative Disorders
ISSN : 2476-2032
Launched : 2016
Journal of Fever
ISSN : 2641-7782
Launched : 2017
JSM Bone Marrow Research
ISSN : 2578-3351
Launched : 2016
JSM Mathematics and Statistics
ISSN : 2578-3173
Launched : 2014
Journal of Autoimmunity and Research
ISSN : 2573-1173
Launched : 2014
JSM Arthritis
ISSN : 2475-9155
Launched : 2016
JSM Head and Neck Cancer-Cases and Reviews
ISSN : 2573-1610
Launched : 2016
JSM General Surgery Cases and Images
ISSN : 2573-1564
Launched : 2016
JSM Anatomy and Physiology
ISSN : 2573-1262
Launched : 2016
JSM Dental Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1548
Launched : 2016
Annals of Emergency Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1017
Launched : 2016
Annals of Mens Health and Wellness
ISSN : 2641-7707
Launched : 2017
Journal of Preventive Medicine and Health Care
ISSN : 2576-0084
Launched : 2018
Journal of Chronic Diseases and Management
ISSN : 2573-1300
Launched : 2016
Annals of Vaccines and Immunization
ISSN : 2378-9379
Launched : 2014
JSM Heart Surgery Cases and Images
ISSN : 2578-3157
Launched : 2016
Annals of Reproductive Medicine and Treatment
ISSN : 2573-1092
Launched : 2016
JSM Brain Science
ISSN : 2573-1289
Launched : 2016
JSM Biomarkers
ISSN : 2578-3815
Launched : 2014
JSM Biology
ISSN : 2475-9392
Launched : 2016
Archives of Stem Cell and Research
ISSN : 2578-3580
Launched : 2014
Annals of Clinical and Medical Microbiology
ISSN : 2578-3629
Launched : 2014
JSM Pediatric Surgery
ISSN : 2578-3149
Launched : 2017
Journal of Memory Disorder and Rehabilitation
ISSN : 2578-319X
Launched : 2016
JSM Tropical Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2578-3165
Launched : 2016
JSM Head and Face Medicine
ISSN : 2578-3793
Launched : 2016
JSM Cardiothoracic Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1297
Launched : 2016
JSM Bone and Joint Diseases
ISSN : 2578-3351
Launched : 2017
JSM Bioavailability and Bioequivalence
ISSN : 2641-7812
Launched : 2017
JSM Atherosclerosis
ISSN : 2573-1270
Launched : 2016
Journal of Genitourinary Disorders
ISSN : 2641-7790
Launched : 2017
Journal of Fractures and Sprains
ISSN : 2578-3831
Launched : 2016
Journal of Autism and Epilepsy
ISSN : 2641-7774
Launched : 2016
Annals of Marine Biology and Research
ISSN : 2573-105X
Launched : 2014
JSM Health Education & Primary Health Care
ISSN : 2578-3777
Launched : 2016
JSM Communication Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3807
Launched : 2016
Annals of Musculoskeletal Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3599
Launched : 2016
Annals of Virology and Research
ISSN : 2573-1122
Launched : 2014
JSM Renal Medicine
ISSN : 2573-1637
Launched : 2016
Journal of Muscle Health
ISSN : 2578-3823
Launched : 2016
JSM Genetics and Genomics
ISSN : 2334-1823
Launched : 2013
JSM Anxiety and Depression
ISSN : 2475-9139
Launched : 2016
Clinical Journal of Heart Diseases
ISSN : 2641-7766
Launched : 2016
Annals of Medicinal Chemistry and Research
ISSN : 2378-9336
Launched : 2014
JSM Pain and Management
ISSN : 2578-3378
Launched : 2016
JSM Women's Health
ISSN : 2578-3696
Launched : 2016
Clinical Research in HIV or AIDS
ISSN : 2374-0094
Launched : 2013
Journal of Endocrinology, Diabetes and Obesity
ISSN : 2333-6692
Launched : 2013
Journal of Substance Abuse and Alcoholism
ISSN : 2373-9363
Launched : 2013
JSM Neurosurgery and Spine
ISSN : 2373-9479
Launched : 2013
Journal of Liver and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2379-0830
Launched : 2014
Journal of Drug Design and Research
ISSN : 2379-089X
Launched : 2014
JSM Clinical Oncology and Research
ISSN : 2373-938X
Launched : 2013
JSM Bioinformatics, Genomics and Proteomics
ISSN : 2576-1102
Launched : 2014
JSM Chemistry
ISSN : 2334-1831
Launched : 2013
Journal of Trauma and Care
ISSN : 2573-1246
Launched : 2014
JSM Surgical Oncology and Research
ISSN : 2578-3688
Launched : 2016
Annals of Food Processing and Preservation
ISSN : 2573-1033
Launched : 2016
Journal of Radiology and Radiation Therapy
ISSN : 2333-7095
Launched : 2013
JSM Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation
ISSN : 2578-3572
Launched : 2016
Annals of Clinical Pathology
ISSN : 2373-9282
Launched : 2013
Annals of Cardiovascular Diseases
ISSN : 2641-7731
Launched : 2016
Journal of Behavior
ISSN : 2576-0076
Launched : 2016
Annals of Clinical and Experimental Metabolism
ISSN : 2572-2492
Launched : 2016
Clinical Research in Infectious Diseases
ISSN : 2379-0636
Launched : 2013
JSM Microbiology
ISSN : 2333-6455
Launched : 2013
Journal of Urology and Research
ISSN : 2379-951X
Launched : 2014
Journal of Family Medicine and Community Health
ISSN : 2379-0547
Launched : 2013
Annals of Pregnancy and Care
ISSN : 2578-336X
Launched : 2017
JSM Cell and Developmental Biology
ISSN : 2379-061X
Launched : 2013
Annals of Aquaculture and Research
ISSN : 2379-0881
Launched : 2014
Clinical Research in Pulmonology
ISSN : 2333-6625
Launched : 2013
Journal of Immunology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2333-6714
Launched : 2013
Annals of Forensic Research and Analysis
ISSN : 2378-9476
Launched : 2014
JSM Biochemistry and Molecular Biology
ISSN : 2333-7109
Launched : 2013
Annals of Breast Cancer Research
ISSN : 2641-7685
Launched : 2016
Annals of Gerontology and Geriatric Research
ISSN : 2378-9409
Launched : 2014
Journal of Sleep Medicine and Disorders
ISSN : 2379-0822
Launched : 2014
JSM Burns and Trauma
ISSN : 2475-9406
Launched : 2016
Chemical Engineering and Process Techniques
ISSN : 2333-6633
Launched : 2013
Annals of Clinical Cytology and Pathology
ISSN : 2475-9430
Launched : 2014
JSM Allergy and Asthma
ISSN : 2573-1254
Launched : 2016
Journal of Neurological Disorders and Stroke
ISSN : 2334-2307
Launched : 2013
Annals of Sports Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2379-0571
Launched : 2014
JSM Sexual Medicine
ISSN : 2578-3718
Launched : 2016
Annals of Vascular Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2378-9344
Launched : 2014
JSM Biotechnology and Biomedical Engineering
ISSN : 2333-7117
Launched : 2013
Journal of Hematology and Transfusion
ISSN : 2333-6684
Launched : 2013
JSM Environmental Science and Ecology
ISSN : 2333-7141
Launched : 2013
Journal of Cardiology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2333-6676
Launched : 2013
JSM Nanotechnology and Nanomedicine
ISSN : 2334-1815
Launched : 2013
Journal of Ear, Nose and Throat Disorders
ISSN : 2475-9473
Launched : 2016
Journal of Pharmacology and Clinical Toxicology
ISSN : 2333-7079
Launched : 2013
Annals of Psychiatry and Mental Health
ISSN : 2374-0124
Launched : 2013
Medical Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology
ISSN : 2333-6439
Launched : 2013
Annals of Pediatrics and Child Health
ISSN : 2373-9312
Launched : 2013
JSM Clinical Pharmaceutics
ISSN : 2379-9498
Launched : 2014
JSM Foot and Ankle
ISSN : 2475-9112
Launched : 2016
JSM Alzheimer's Disease and Related Dementia
ISSN : 2378-9565
Launched : 2014
Journal of Addiction Medicine and Therapy
ISSN : 2333-665X
Launched : 2013
Journal of Veterinary Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2378-931X
Launched : 2013
Annals of Public Health and Research
ISSN : 2378-9328
Launched : 2014
Annals of Orthopedics and Rheumatology
ISSN : 2373-9290
Launched : 2013
Journal of Clinical Nephrology and Research
ISSN : 2379-0652
Launched : 2014
Annals of Community Medicine and Practice
ISSN : 2475-9465
Launched : 2014
Annals of Biometrics and Biostatistics
ISSN : 2374-0116
Launched : 2013
JSM Clinical Case Reports
ISSN : 2373-9819
Launched : 2013
Journal of Cancer Biology and Research
ISSN : 2373-9436
Launched : 2013
Journal of Surgery and Transplantation Science
ISSN : 2379-0911
Launched : 2013
Journal of Dermatology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2373-9371
Launched : 2013
JSM Gastroenterology and Hepatology
ISSN : 2373-9487
Launched : 2013
Annals of Nursing and Practice
ISSN : 2379-9501
Launched : 2014
JSM Dentistry
ISSN : 2333-7133
Launched : 2013
Author Information X