Loading

Journal of Human Nutrition and Food Science

The Relationship between Dietary Preference and Food Waste: An Application of the Theory of Planned Behaviour

Review Article | Open Access | Volume 13 | Issue 1
Article DOI :

  • 1. Psychologist PhD Candidate School of Health and Biomedical Sciences, Australia
+ Show More - Show Less
Corresponding Authors
Patricia Jackson, Psychologist PhD Candidate School of Health and Biomedical Sciences RMIT University, Plenty Road, Bundoora VIC 3083, Australia
Abstract

Food waste is a critical environmental issue, with household food waste representing the largest contributor to overall food loss. While previous research has linked food waste behaviour to psychosocial factors, the influence of dietary preference on food waste remains underexplored. This study applied the Theory of Planned Behaviour (TPB) to examine whether diet type (vegan, vegetarian, or omnivore) predicts household food waste behaviour. A total of 363 participants completed an online survey measuring food-related routines, household skills, TPB constructs, and pro-environmental attitudes. Results indicated that household food waste was significantly predicted by perceived behavioural control, shopping and planning routines, and household skills. Pro-environmental attitudes were weakly associated with reduced food waste but significantly predicted intentions not to waste food. Vegans reported significantly stronger pro-environmental attitudes and less food waste than omnivores, with vegetarians falling in between. These findings support an extended TPB model and highlight the relevance of dietary identity in shaping food waste behaviour. Targeting food-related routines and enhancing perceived control may improve the effectiveness of food waste reduction campaigns, particularly when tailored to dietary groups. 

Keywords: Dietary Preference; Food Waste; Planned Behaviour; Psychosocial factors

Introduction

Food Waste

The population of the world is growing exponentially, and with this growth comes an increased demand for resources (such as fossil fuels, freshwater, and food) and an increase in waste production and environmental degradation [1]. These increases are further exacerbated by consumption habits, patterns of social organization, and resource management [2,3], concerns about how to sustainably feed a growing population often places the emphasis on increasing production whilst reducing the use of natural resources and cutting carbon emissions [4]. However, one way of strengthening global food security, while reducing the environmental footprint of food, lies in a different approach: cutting the vast amounts of food wasted every year [4-6].

 In the various stages of the food supply chain, large quantities (approximately 33% a year) of food available for human consumption are lost or wasted; this includes uneaten food products that are thrown away by consumers (also known as avoidable food waste) [7,8]. The amount of energy, water, land, and other resources needed to produce food that is then wasted, makes this issue critical [6]. For example, 25% of water used in agriculture is used to grow food that it ultimately being wasted (Department of Agriculture, Water, and the Environment, 2020). Furthermore, food waste has become a leading contributor to climate change, globally generating roughly eight percent of human-caused greenhouse gas emissions [5-9], the aim of this study was to examine household food waste determinants with a focus on the avoidable portion of food waste. This food waste is defined as food and drink, which was edible at some point before being thrown out [8].

An unintentional result of our biological need to procure, prepare and consume food for survival is that a portion of food intended for our consumption becomes waste [10]. Buying food, not consuming it, and then discarding it is becoming a huge global and social issue, with household food waste having a detrimental effect on the planet [11]. According to the Rabobank 2020 Food Waste report, Australians are now wasting 12.7% of their weekly grocery shop. In Australia alone, this waste is costing the average household $1043 per year and totally $10.3 billion nationally. One in ten individuals around the world go to bed hungry yet, developed nations (including USA and Australia) waste about 1.3 billion tonnes of edible food per year [12,13].

These statistics on food waste highlight the need to reduce the amount of food being wasted. Reducing food waste at the consumer stage is highly feasible and therefore critical and necessary to prevent further climate change [11-15]. There is a modest yet rapidly growing body of literature on food waste, however, given its complex nature, the evidence of the drivers of food wastage and barriers to its reduction remain unclear [11]. Although studying consumer behaviours towards food waste is a relatively new area of research, understanding these complex and diverse motivations is critical to identifying effective ways to reduce food waste [7-16].

Qualitative research has mostly dominated the investigation for determinants of food waste [16-18]. This line of inquiry provides important insight into the potential motivational precursors of food waste behaviour, by focusing on variables such as attitudes, social norms, knowledge, and intentions [19]. Previous research indicated that consumers consider throwing away food as improper behaviour, marking it as a significant predictor of an individual’s intention to waste food [7-21]. These studies have advanced the field further; however, it remains unclear whether diet preference (an individual’s choice of eating an omnivorous or plant-based diet) might have an impact on the amount of food a person wastes.

 The aim of this study is to build on the existing knowledge of the psycho-social predictors of food waste by testing additional factors that may predict this complex behaviour. Specifically, this study examined the role diet preference has on household food waste behaviour. The consumption of meat is a complex eating behaviour, with overconsumption resulting in harmful impacts on both health and the environment [22]. Global agriculture production is having a significant negative environmental impact, with animal agriculture being responsible for an estimated 11% of Australia’s greenhouse gas emissions, 54% of its land use, and 52% of the nation’s freshwater usage [23,24]. Most vegetarians in the West are not raised as such, however due to these harmful effects on the environment, some individuals choose to convert from an omnivorous diet to a vegetarian or vegan diet [25,26]. Vegetarianism can be defined as the avoidance of eating animal flesh, while those that avoid all animal products, and their byproducts are denoted as vegans [26]. The number of Australians following these diets is unclear, although research suggests that there may be up to 2.5 million vegans and vegetarians in Australia in 2022, with approximately 6% of the population describing themselves as strict vegans [22-27]. Research has demonstrated that plant-based diets appear environmentally better than meat-based ones, however it is unclear if undertaking a specific diet will result in less food being wasted [27].

Theory of Planned Behaviour

Proposed by [28], the Theory of Planned Behaviour (TPB) has been widely and successfully applied to predict household food waste behaviour [7,21]. The model proposes that behaviour is directly influenced by intentions, which are influenced by three conceptually independent antecedents (attitudes, subjective norms and perceived behavioural control) that determine the strength of a person’s intention to carry out a behaviour [10]. In previous research, the range of explained variance intentions explains in relation to food waste behaviour varies from quite [7-29], noted attitudes (through intentions) were the strongest determinants of food waste behaviour. Whilst some research posits that the core values of vegetarians and omnivores differ, other researchers have found less dramatic differences in their beliefs [26-30]. Environmental reasons (including unnecessary waste) are a main motivator for people to convert to a plant-based diet [26-31]. It has also been reported that omnivores are less concerned regarding the environmental impacts of their food compared to vegans [22-33], Despite having a significant difference in the environmental consequences of their food choices, the extent to which omnivores, vegetarians and vegans differ in their concern for the environment and their attitude toward food waste has not been thoroughly investigated. This research will investigate the role of attitudes in food waste.

Subjective norms are also theorised to indirectly impact behaviour through their impact on intentions. In a study by Gatersleben [34], environmental values and identities were shown to be good predictors of pro-environmental behaviours. Environmental identity is a way a person has formed their self-concept as a sense of connection to another collective identity [35]. If becoming a vegan for environmental reasons has created a social and environmental identity for that individual, then they may be less likely to behave in ways that would hurt the environment [36]. In relation to food waste, morality, guilt, and environmental concern are considered key subjective norms [6-37], noted that subjective norms were significantly related to intention only regarding the amount of protein being wasted (i.e.,meat and fish) and none of the other food groups. That study, however, did not discriminate between the different dietary food groups. Visschers et al. [19], confirmed the suggestion of Quested et al. [6], that due to the discrete nature of food waste behaviour (typically only ever seen by the generator) subjective norms have only been reported to moderately influence intention [16-21].

Perceived behavioural control is thought to both directly predict behaviour and to indirectly predict behaviour through its impact on intentions. Perceived behavioural control has been found to both influence intention and directly influence behaviour [7-21]. In some research, it has been found that perceived behavioural control has a large indirect relationship to food waste behaviour through planning and shopping routines [21]. Perceived behavioural control affects intentions related to situations such as large sizes of food packaging, unexpected meals outside of the home and the conflict between food provisioning and fussy eaters [17-38], found that convenience was a barrier for going vegan/vegetarian for many consumers. If accessibility differs between the three dietary groups, then it can be hypothesised that their perceived behavioural control will also have a differential influence on food waste. The present study aims to investigate this potential, but untested, predictor of food waste.

Limitations of the TPB

Generally, the more favourable the attitude, the stronger the subjective norms, and the greater the perceived behavioural control, the stronger the individual’s intention will be to behave (not waste food). However, the relative importance of each conceptual antecedent is expected to vary across behaviours and situations [28]. Since its conception, the TPB has proved to be a powerful explanatory approach for predicting complex human behaviour. However, there are some limitations and criticisms of the TPB model. It has been noted that it is challenging to apply the TPB to food waste as it is a complex behaviour that is made up of multiple behaviours rather than a singular behaviour [6 -29], further criticises the model at its inability to deal with irrational behaviours and habits which food waste behaviour oftentimes can be. For example, saving money and other personal concerns, elicit a strong motivation to reduce food waste [7-39], both in quantitative research [7-41], and qualitative research [16]. However, this does not explain why (despite the cost) some people are still wasting food and some are not. Researchers found that people who voice a “high environmental concern have a marked aversion towards wasting food” [11-42]. Considering the massive impact household food waste is having on our environment, an environmental concern has the potential to be a predictor of food waste behaviour. Therefore, this study included pro-environmental attitudes as a separate measure to explore the possibility of explaining food waste behaviour further.

Pro-Environmental Attitudes

 Traditionally, the TPB focuses on the attitudes towards the specific behaviour (in this case, household food waste). The inclusion of pro-environmental attitudes in the current study, is an attempt to give more depth into the attitudes that drive intention and behaviour. To understand the ideology of the human/environment relationship, previous research has focused on environmental awareness and concern. The concept of a pro-environmental psychological attitude was first developed in the 1970’s [43], and was defined as a person’s inclination to have concern for human impact on the natural environment [44]. As mentioned, in terms of the relationship between attitude, intention and behaviour; attitude strength is seen as the strongest predictor of intention [16-29]. To make a prediction that is consistent with the TPB, pro-environmental attitude was expected to positively correlate with pro-environmental behaviours [28]. Research has consistently shown that pro-environmental attitude is positively related to pro-environmental behaviour [45,46]. This reflects the notion that people who have more environmental concerns will act in ways to protect it [47,48], Under an umbrella of ecologically responsible behaviours, reducing waste is considered a pro-environmental behaviour [49], therefore this research will attempt to determine if pro environmental attitudes are positively related to reduced food waste.

Some studies have suggested that concerns about the environmental impacts of food waste have only minimal impact on an individual’s motivations to reduce their wasteful behaviour [39]. This weak association of food waste and environmental concern could be explained by a lack of awareness and knowledge between food waste and its ecological impacts [6-21]. This lack of knowledge was also highlighted in a qualitative study by Watson and Meah [18], where none of their respondents suggested any sort of link between greenhouse gas emissions and food. However, in environmentally educated households it was found that these consumers wasted less food [37]. Given this finding, it is further hypothesised that omnivores, vegetarians, and vegans are likely to differ in food waste (behaviour and intention) and in pro-environmental attitudes as a reflection of the disconnection between what a person eats and the environment. The rationale of this study was to build on existing research and expand the understanding of food waste behaviour with a focus on the role of pro-environmental attitudes and dietary preference as motivators of food waste in the context of the TPB. The following was hypothesised:

Hypothesis 1: In accordance with the TPB, it was predicted that intentions to not waste food would be predicted by higher scores on attitudes, subjective norms, and perceived behavioural control of household food waste.

Hypothesis 2: Pro-environmental attitudes will predict household food waste behaviours.

Hypothesis 3: Vegans will score higher on pro-environmental attitudes than omnivores and vegetarians.

Hypothesis 4: Vegans will report less food wastage than omnivores and vegetarians.

Method

Procedure

Ethics approval was first obtained by CQ University Human Research Ethics Committee before data collection (approval number 2020-110). The study procedure consisted of participants responding to a web-based survey developed in the Qualtrics software [50]. Before recruitment, a power analysis was conducted in G*Power to determine needed sample size. Hypothesis 1 (which tested the predictive validity of the TPB [28], was estimated to have the smallest anticipated effect. The needed sample size for this hypothesis was calculated for a multiple linear regression with a medium effect size of 0.15, error probability 0.04, power 0.8 with two predictors (behaviour and intention). Correlational studies show that intentions are reliably associated with behaviour [51], therefore a medium (conservative) effect size was chosen. There were 363 participants who completed the online questionnaire (conducted from October 2020 till November 2020).

Participants were recruited online through a Facebook group created to engage omnivores, vegetarians, and vegans. This targeting recruitment was conducted to ensure that the relatively low prevalence group in the general population of those with plant-based diets would be represented in the sample to an extent such that comparisons could be conducted with sufficient cell sizes. A link to the survey was posted in the group with a plain language statement offering a clear description of the research aims and the incentive. As reward for participation, respondents went into a draw to win a $150 voucher.

Participants

There were 363 participants who completed the online questionnaire which far exceeded the minimum required sample of 68 (see power analysis above). Participants were required to be older than the age of 18 years, which was the only exclusionary criterion for being included in this study. The sample contained persons aged between 18 and 74 years of age and vastly more females compared to males (~83% vs ~17%). The socio-demographic characteristics of the final sample are presented in Table 1.

Materials

The first part of the survey consisted of items to collect demographic characteristic data; items for the participants to indicate age, gender, socioeconomic and employment status. A single item was used for participants to self-identify as either a vegan, vegetarian, or omnivore.

Pro-environmental Attitudes

 Pro-environmental attitudes were assessed with the Ecologically Conscious Consumer Behaviour (ECCB) Scale [52]. Subscale of Pro-environmental Attitudes. This sub-test consisted of 35 items recorded on a 5-point Likert scale (anchored by ‘strongly disagree’ [one point] and ‘strongly agree’ [five points]) to gauge attitudes towards the environment such as: “Overconsumption is highly responsible for the environments destruction” and “Natural resources must be preserved, even if people have to do without some products.” Following reverse-coding where relevant, responses to items were summed/averaged to form a total pro-environmental attitude score for each participant. In its entirety, the scale had poor internal reliability (<.50). Following a 2-step factor analysis, those items that poorly loaded onto a single factor were omitted (k = 9) and those with loadings more than .32 were included in the revised scale score (k = 29). The final scale had a Cronbach’s alpha of .88.

TPB: Food Waste Behaviour

Food waste was assessed with the self-reported consumer food waste scale based on the TPB. This scale was first developed by Stefan et al. [21], and then replicated and validated by Stancu et al. [7], Stancu et al. [7], and Stefan et al. [21], utilised a mixture of 3-point, 5-point and 7-point Likert scales. The current study adapted the scale so that it used a consistent 5-point scale throughout. To accommodate for vegans and vegetarians, the inclusion of plant-milks, faux meat and fish were included in questions that specifically related to food. Past research in pro-environmental behaviour operationalized habits as past behaviour (Russell et al. [29]. Past food waste behaviour was operationalized as an indicator of food waste habit. The questions varied in their wording, but most included the instructions “thinking about your household” please rate your disagreement (strongly disagree [1]) vs agreement (strongly agree [5]).

1. Food waste behaviour (self-reported): Food waste was defined as, “all food and drink which at some point prior to being thrown away was edible.” The items were self-reported and four sub-categories of food were utilised, these were dairy (including vegan dairy products), fresh fruit and vegetables, meat and fish (including faux meat and fish) and bakery products. Responses from this portion of the questionnaire were summed to form a total food waste score for each individual. Following this, the scale was further comprised of items that measured participants’ intentions not to waste food, food-related routines, household skills, attitudes towards food waste, moral and injunctive norms, and perceived behavioural control in line with the TPB. Responses from this portion of the questionnaire were worded as “How much (insert food sub-category) is thrown away in your household of what you buy/grow, in a regular week” with the scale: hardly any (1), less than a tenth (less than 10%) (2), more than a tenth but less than a quarter (between 10% and 20%) (3), more than a quarter but less than a half (between 25% and 50%) (4), more than half (more than 50%) (5).

2. TPB: Intention not to waste food: As wasting food was considered an odd purposeful behaviour, intention not to waste food was measured rather than an intention to, which was introduced by “Please answer the following question thinking about the near future (e.g next one/two weeks) and your household”. The intention was measured using three items on a 5-point Likert scale such as “I intend not to throw food away” on a scale from strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (5) developed by Stancu et al. [7], following the TPB [28], guidelines. The items from this portion were summed to form a total intention score.

3. TPB: Attitudes towards food waste: General attitudes towards food waste were measured using 3 items rated on 5-point scales. One item referred to loading the environment with one’s household food waste “In my opinion loading the environment with my waste is…” (from not at all harmful/negative [1] to extremely harmful/negative [5]), while the other two items referred to throwing away food (measured on a 5-point Likert scale from avoidable [1] to unavoidable [5]). These items were developed by Stancu et al. [7], in accordance with the TPB [28], guidelines.

4. TPB: Social (Moral) norms: Moral norms were measured with a 3-item 5-point Likert scale. Each item that measured from strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (5) and were questions such as “Wasting food would…give me a bad conscience.” These questions were adapted from prior literature on food related behaviours and environmental behaviours by Stancu et al.

5. TPB: Social (Injunctive) norms: Injunctive norms were measured with a 4-item 5point Likert scale referring to what one ought to do regarding food waste in general (“One should never waste any food” and food waste in relation to the environment (“One should not load the environment with food waste”). These represented social injunctive norms. The items were measured from strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (5).

6. TPB: Perceived behavioural control: Perceived behavioural control was measured with a 3-item 5-point Likert scale that was developed in line with the TPB [28], and prior studies on household food waste [53]; 7 Two items asked how avoidable/unavoidable food waste was in general (avoidable [1], unavoidable [5]) and in relation to the environment. One item asked about the ease/difficulty of not wasting food (easy [1], difficult [5]).

Antecedents to food waste

1. Shopping routines: This 3-item scale on shopping routines regarded the purchase of larger amounts of food than needed; such as “We often buy unintended products when shopping”. These items were measured on a 5-point Likert scale from strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (5). It was developed based on shopping routine measures in previous literature [7-21].

2. Leftovers reuse routines: Reuse of leftovers routines were measured with a 3-item 5point Likert scale (from strongly disagree [1] to strongly agree [5]) that referred to the storage and reuse of leftovers. Responses were made from questions such as “The leftovers are stored in appropriate conditions, so they last.” These items were developed by Stancu et al. [7], based on previous literature [53,54].

3. Planning routines: Planning referred to shopping trips and future meals. These two items were adapted from previous studies on consumer food waste [7-54]. The questions “The shopping trips are normally planned in advanced (shopping lists are made, inventories are checked, etc)” and “The home meals are usually planned for a couple days ahead” were measured on a 5-point Likert scale from strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (5).

4. Household skills: Five items on a 5-point Likert scale such as “How would you rate your households skills on planning the meals” (from very poor [1] to very good [5]) were used to measure perceived capabilities to deal with household skills, they referred to how poor/good people perceive their skills related to several specific household related activities.

These items were developed based on previous studies on household food waste [7-54].

The published reliability for these constructs and the reliability calculated can be found in Table 2.

Data Analyses

Of the 460 respondents that commenced the survey, 97 participants did not provide sufficient data to allow for inclusion in the analyses.

Hypothesis 1

 To test our first hypothesis that the TPB was a sufficient theory to predict food waste, the data analyses were conducted in two stages, First, assumptions of linearity, homoscedasticity and normality were all measured and met. Secondly, the conceptual models were tested. A multiple linear regression was run on intentions, shopping routines, leftovers reuse routines, planning routines, household skills, attitudes, moral norms, perceived behavioural control, and injunctive norms. Next, a linear regression was used to determine if intention predicted behaviour. After this, a multiple linear regression was applied to determine if the main conceptual components (attitudes, norms and perceived behavioural control) predicted intention. Finally, a linear regression was performed to determine if perceived behavioural control had a direct effect on food waste behaviour.

Hypothesis 2

To test the second hypothesis, a Pearson Correlation was used to examine the relationship between pro-environmental attitudes and food waste. Before determining if a correlation exists between pro-environmental attitudes and household food waste, the researchers first checked for any assumptions visually. The data appeared to violate the assumption of heteroscedasticity, so the researchers used Spearman’s rho to determine an association between the two variables. To explore pro-environmental attitudes within the scope of the TPB, a second correlation was performed to investigate the relationship between pro-environmental attitudes and intentions not to waste food.

Hypothesis 3

An independent between groups analysis of variance (ANOVA) was planned initially to analyse the difference of pro-environmental attitudes as a function of the categorical independent variable of diet preference. All the assumptions were tested and Levene’s test of homogeneity of variance was violated. Due to this assumption, a Kruskall-Wallis test was conducted to compare the groups.

Hypothesis 4

To generate data for the fourth hypothesis, first the data were inspected visually to check the assumption of linearity and heteroscedasticity. An independent between groups ANOVA was planned initially to analyse the difference of food waste as a function of the categorical independent variable diet preference. This produced a significant result for the Levene’s test of homogeneity of variance (F[2,360] = 5.01, p < .01). Due to the violation of this assumption, a Kruskal-Wallis test was conducted to compare the groups. An independent between-groups ANOVA and a subsequent Tukey’s post hoc test was run to investigate the differences between vegans, vegetarians, and omnivores on the conceptual model of the TPB.

Table 1:

Table 1: Socio-demographic characteristics of the sample (N = 363)

               

N

%

 

Gender                                                          

Female                                                           

 

303

 

83.5

 

Male                                                               

Other

Occupation

59

1

16.3

0.3

 

Working full time                                          

157

43.3

 

Working part-time/casual                              

Not currently employed/ looking for work

Retired

Homemaker

Disabled (not working because of permanent or temporary disability)

Student

109

18

25

24

12

 

18

30

5

6.9

6.6

3.3

 

5

 

Household Income                                       

$0 - $18,200

$18,201 - $45,000

 

34

66

 

9.4

18.2

 

 

$45,000 - $120,00

$120,001 - $180,000

$180,000 and over

 

Age

Pro-Environmental Attitude

(Maximum Score 120)

148

76

39

40.8

20.9

10.7

 

Mean/SD

40.3/13.9

101.8/9.5

Table 2

Table 2: Previously published reliability and present reliability scores

Factors and items                                           

Published CR

Present CA

Food Waste Behaviour                                  

.85

.73

Intention not to waste food                            

.94

.82

Shopping routines                                          

Leftovers reuse routines

Planning routines

.56

.59

.75

.63

.55

.91

Household skills                                            

.86

.82

Attitudes towards food waste                        

Moral Norms

Perceived behavioural control

Injunctive Norms

.82

.87

.80

.70

.85

.82

.66

.73

Table 3

Table 3: Results of the Multiple Linear Regression on Food Waste. Overall Model (n=363)

Variables                                    

Β

SE

Beta

t

p

Intention not to waste food        

-.07

.06

-.05

-1.12

.262

Shopping routines                      

Leftovers reuse routines

Planning routines

.12

-.10

.15

.04

.06

.06

.13

-.08

.14

3.01

-1.66

2.70

.003

.097

.007

Household skills                         

-.21

.04

-.29

-4.76

.000

Attitudes towards food waste     

.02

.04

.04

.65

.519

Moral Norms

-.02

.05

-.02

-.45

.656

Injunctive Norms

-.07

.05

-.07

-1.42

.158

Perceived behavioural control

-.89

.12

-.37

-7.60

.000

 

The results show that, shopping routines (β = .12 p < .001), planning routines (β = .15, p < 0.01), household skills (β = -.21, p < .001), and perceived behavioural control (β = -.89, p < .001) were significant predictors of food waste behaviour in the overall model.

Food waste was significantly predicted by intention (F[8,362] = 7.39, p < .001), with an R² of .42.

Table 4

Table 4: Results of the Multiple Linear Regression on Intention. Overall Model (n=363)

Variables                                    

β

SE

Beta

t

p

Attitudes towards food waste     

0.81

.04

.14

2.28

.023

Moral Norms                              

Injunctive Norms

Perceived Behavioural Control

.13

.15

.39

.04

.04

.09

.16

.20

.22

3.01

3.76

4.30

.003

.000

.000

 

 Food waste was significantly predicted by perceived behavioural control (F[1,362] =  63.35, p < .001), with an R² of .56.

Table 5

Table 5: Means and Standard deviations for Pro-environmental Attitudes

  Omnivore (O)  Vegetarian (Vt)  Vegan (Vg)   
Variable  Mean (SD)  Mean (SD)  Mean (SD)  Tukey outcome 
         
Pro-Environmental  98.72 (9.76)  104.61 (7.07)  105.46 (8.25)  O < Vt, Vg 
Attitudes  Vt = Vg       

 

 

Table 6

Table 6: Means and Standard deviations for Food Waste

 

Variable

Omnivore (O)

Mean (SD)

Vegetarian (Vt)

Mean (SD)

Vegan (Vg)

Mean (SD)

 

Tukey outcome

Food Waste

7.65 (2.38)

7.62 (2.81)

 6.65 (1.88)

Vg < Vt, O

Vt = O

 

There was a statistically significant difference between vegans and omnivores (p < .001) with vegans wasting significantly less food than omnivores.

Table 7

Table 7: Independent between-groups ANOVA for each construct of the TPB

Variables                                    

F

Sig

Intention not to waste food        

9.84

.000

Shopping routines                      

Leftovers reuse routines

Planning routines

.42

5.00

1.14

.661

.029

.320

Household skills                         

2.52

.082

Attitudes towards food waste     

Moral Norms

Perceived behavioural control

Injunctive Norms

10.86

9.54

7.52

8.95

.000

.000

.001

.000

Results

Hypothesis 1

Food waste was significantly predicted by the overall model (F [9,353] = 28.75, p < .01), with an R² of .42. These results can be found in Table 3.

Intention to not waste food was significantly predicted by attitudes not to waste food, norms, and perceived behavioural control (F [4,362] = 34.46, p < .001), with an R² of .53. The results for this are found in Table 4.

Hypothesis 2

There was a very weak, significant, negative relationship between pro-environmental attitudes and food waste (r = -.16, p = .001) therefore, it can be concluded that there is a significant correlation between pro-environmental attitudes and food waste in the population and we accept our second hypothesis, although note the size of the effect is small.

There was a moderate, significant, positive relationship between pro-environmental attitudes and intentions to not waste food (r = .28, p < .001) therefore, it can be concluded that there is a significant correlation between pro-environmental attitudes and intentions not to waste food in the population.

Hypothesis 3

The independent between-groups ANOVA on pro-environmental attitudes yielded a statistically significant effect, F(2,360) = 24.46, p < 0.001. Thus, the null hypothesis of no differences between the means was rejected. A Tukey’s post hoc showed that the significant differences were represented between omnivores and vegans and omnivores and vegetarians. There was no significant difference between vegans and vegetarians. Table 5 below presents the means, standard deviations for each diet preference group on pro-environmental attitudes.

Hypothesis 4

The independent between-groups ANOVA on food waste yielded a statistically significant effect (F [2,360] = 8.56, p < .001, η 2 = .04). The significant Levene’s indicated the use of Kruskal-Wallis test. Household food waste behaviour was significantly different for the three dietary preference groups, H (2) = 17.28, p < .001. The pairwise comparison shows that there is no significant difference in food waste behaviour between vegetarians and omnivores (p = .496) or vegetarians and vegans (p = .098). The means and standard deviations for each diet preference can be found in Table 6.

The results of independent between-groups ANOVA testing dietary preference on the conceptual model of the TPB can be found in Table 7.

Discussion

The current study aimed to build on previous research to expand an understanding of food waste behaviour by utilising the TPB [28], The study’s first hypothesis was supported, the TPB demonstrated ability to predict household food waste behaviour. The second hypothesis that pro-environmental attitudes would significantly predict household food waste was supported. The third hypothesis that vegans would report higher on pro-environmental attitudes than vegetarians and omnivores was partially supported. The vegans/vegetarians differed significantly from omnivores but did not differ significantly from each other when it came to environmental concern. The fourth hypothesis was supported, vegans reported the least amount of food waste whilst omnivores reported the most food waste. These results confirm predictions that there is a significant relationship between pro-environmental attitudes, dietary preference, and food waste.

Food Waste Predictors

 In accordance with previous research, this study found evidence to support the growing literature on the predictive utility of the TPB. Those who reported a higher intention to avoid wasting food also reported less food waste in their households. Intention to reduce household food waste was further predicted by attitudes, subjective norms, and perceived behavioural control. Attitudes and subjective norms were not direct significant predictors of food waste behaviour (when included with the extra determinants from Stancu et al. [7], questionnaire). This result was in contrast with previous research as Graham-Rowe et al. [16], Stefan et al. [21], and Visschers et al. [19], who found attitude to be one of (if not the strongest) predictors of intention. It was theorised that social norms may be a poor predictor of food waste behaviour; as household food waste typically has less visibility to other people when compared to other pro-environmental behaviours [29]. Perceived behavioural control had a significant effect on food waste both indirectly through intention and directly on the behaviour. This indicated that individuals who feel confident in their ability to reduce their household food waste, were more likely to intend to reduce their food waste. Similarly, to Bissing-Olson et al. [44], and Werf et al. [10], perceived behavioural control was the strongest predictor of intentions and self-reported household food waste behaviour.

Pro-environmental Attitudes on Predicting Food Wastage

The hypothesis that pro-environmental attitudes would predict food wastage was supported. Several studies have revealed that people with high levels of environmental attitudes tend to act more pro-environmentally [49-57], as was the case in the current study. Pro-environmental attitudes were a significant predictor of both intentions not to waste food and food waste behaviour.

Despite the significant correlation, pro-environmental attitudes and attitudes towards food waste were not the strongest predictors of intention or food waste behaviour when compared to other conceptual antecedents in the model (such as household & food provisioning skills and perceived behavioural control). The benefit of including the extra variables into the TPB [28] model, highlighted the role of habits and domestic skills as strong predictors of household food waste rather than attitudes. Stern et al. [58], found that conservation behaviour was likely to occur with less conscious control and more in a habitual fashion. This finding could be result of several psychological considerations such as personal values [59], and environmental involvement [60]; factors that were further explained by dietary preference.

Omnivores, Vegetarians, Vegans; Environmental Attitudes and Food Waste

 A statistically significant difference in environmental attitudes were found between omnivores, vegetarians, and vegans. Reports that omnivores were less concerned regarding the environmental impacts of their food choices [22-33], appears to be related to their overall ecological concern. Furthermore, vegans reported significantly less food wastage than omnivores and vegetarians. There are several factors that could contribute to this significant relationship. Although attitudes were not a significant predictor of food waste within the overall model, when partitioned, vegans’ pro-environmental attitudes towards food waste did significantly differ compared to omnivores. This weak association was previously highlighted by Watson & Meah [18], who found respondents did not link their food choices to ecological impacts [18]. Various studies have observed an underestimation of the ecological impact of eating meat which may imply a lack of knowledge about the (extra) environmental consequences of meat consumption [61-63]. Williams et al. [37], found that environmental education led to less food waste. It could be theorized that omnivores have less knowledge about the environmental impact of their choice of food and its related inherent waste. If a motivating factor to becoming vegan is environmental concern [31], then it can be stipulated that vegans may have a more thorough understanding of the consequences associated with food waste and would therefore have a significantly different (pro-environmental) attitude and waste profile to omnivores.

The literature further posits that negative attitudes towards food waste are more financial than environmental [6- 18. Povey et al. [64], indicates that for some there is a financial barrier when it comes to diet choice, with the vegan diet being suggested as the most expensive of them all [38-64]. It is well known that plant-based alternatives may include a higher price tag. The high cost of meat substitutes, plant milks and other specialist products could potentially elicit a stronger (financial, as well as pro-environmental) motivation for a vegan (or vegetarians) to consume them rather than waste them. Indicating that omnivores are more prone to throw away food they perceive to be less valuable, when compared to vegans who are less inclined to do so, as they value their food more.

Consistent with a greater pro-environmental attitude, vegans reported significantly less food wastage than omnivores and vegetarians. Having an awareness of the ecological and economic consequences of food waste is strongly related to food-related routines [7]. Highlighted in the current study, household food related routines and skills, and perceived behavioural control were all significant predictors of food waste. Intentions, attitudes, moral norms, injunctive norms, perceived behavioural control and leftovers reuse routines were significantly different between omnivores and vegans. Many consumers have reported that a barrier to going vegan is convenience - due to difficult dish preparation and lack of eating out options [38]. Previous research has found that unexpected meals outside of the home and provisioning for fussy eaters to be determinants of food waste [17-37]. If vegans are the “fussy eaters” and have accumulated the knowledge and effort required to buy and prepare their meals, they could be expected to not waste something that has taken them more time to arrange. Furthermore, if they lack places to eat out, then they will have a better awareness of what is in their homes to be used for consumption.

The shelf-life of vegan products are typically lengthier than their animal product equivalents [65].This could indicate why vegans wasted less food and differed significantly from omnivores in relation to leftovers-reuse, as a question of hygiene needs to be taken into consideration. It can be stipulated that individuals would be wary of eating left-over animal derived products as the potential risk of food poisoning is higher [66].

Omnivores differed significantly to vegetarians and vegans on moral norms, however vegetarians and vegans did not significantly differ. Quested et al. [6], and Watson & Meah [18], found that morals and guilt were key subjective norms in the context of food waste. It could be posited that due to the level of morality a vegan or vegetarian hold over their food consumption this would translate into their thoughts on food wastage. If a vegan or vegetarian thinks it is not right to “waste a life” to eat food, perhaps it is just as plausible that they think it is morally reprehensible to waste any food. In a study by Gatersleben et al. [34], environmental values and identities were shown to be good predictors of pro-environmental behaviours. Although food waste was not included in this study, the significant difference between omnivores and vegans/vegetarians on injunctive norms could potentially be related to the latter maintaining a pro-environmental personal identity [34]. If vegans have formed a pro-environmental identity, they would be less likely to engage in behaviours that hurt the environment, which would explain the difference in norms and food waste behaviour between vegans and omnivores.

Practical Implications

There is an increasing need to decrease food waste due its environmental and social consequences. The use of a combined TPB in the present study provides useful information for designing campaigns aimed at reducing food waste at the household level. Stancu et al. [7], and Stefan et al. [21], both recommend campaigns target consumer food planning and shopping routines. The results of the current study also strongly support the approach to targeting households, specifically, their routines associated with shopping and leftover reuse.

Shopping skills, planning routines, household skills and perceived behavioural control were the strongest determinants of reported household food waste, a finding aligned with past literature [7]. Despite the significant association that intention had with food waste, its predictive magnitude of food waste behaviour was weaker when the other predictors were included - this is consistent with the findings of Stancu et al. [7], and Werf et al. [10]. This finding could be explained by the postulate that food waste is embedded in routines for food provisioning and not “driven by conscious intentions” [21]. Debatably, no one intends to deliberately waste food that at some point was available for consumption. In the context of this study and in relation to previous research, food management skills (such as shopping skills, planning routines, household skills) and perceived behavioural control would be better predictors of this behaviour and where initiatives to reduce waste should focus their attention.

 In the last decade, numerous phone apps have been developed with the aim to reducing food waste. Utilising the power of technology, these apps are designed to tackle food waste from various strategies such as leftover education, managing food once it has been purchased, and preparing economical meals that are made up of items already located in an individual’s fridge [67]. To make an impact, it is necessary to provide advice to improve people’s knowledge about food waste and the skills to improve their food routines for example, household economic campaigns. Perceived improvement in planning, leftover reuse and shopping skills will indirectly result in lower food waste. Improving individual’s food literacy by providing them information would allow them to improve their household habits. Providing people with recipes for reuse or repurposing of leftovers would lead to less food being wasted. A way to strengthen these campaigns would be to integrate a change in consumer attitudes.

Limitations

Psychologists have mostly approached the investigation of food waste behaviour through applying the TPB [7-21]. Despite its strong empirical support in explaining environmentally relevant behaviours, the TPB receives criticism due to it underrepresentation of non-cognitive determinants of behaviour, specifically habits and emotions [29]. The TPB assumes that individuals make reasoned and rational choices, however, due to the limited public visibility of household food waste, social normative drivers of food waste are likely to be less important. Therefore, the TPB may not be sufficient to predict household food waste as it does not account processes including habits, routines, and emotions [29].

Future research

The “who and why” of food waste is a relatively new area of research. From an extensive literature review, this exploratory research is the first to determine if dietary preference was a determinant of food waste. Future research should qualitatively or quantitatively determine the specific differences between these three dietary groups. Considering antecedents of moral and injunctive norms would also provide further insight into what makes some people waste food and others not. As wasting food is primarily a socially undesirable habit not under volitional control, future research may aim to eliminate this factor by utilising cameras, waste caddies and scales to get a more quantifiable measure of what and how much food is being measured.

Conclusion

In summary, the current study explored dietary preference and pro-environmental attitudes on household food waste through the utilisation of the TPB model. Findings supported the utility of this model, however, some constructs of the TPB were not sufficient to explain food waste behaviour. The importance of including household skills and routines is highlighted in this study. The findings of this study also provide evidence that vegans differ from omnivores in their food waste behaviours, as vegans reported the least amount of food waste whilst omnivores reported the most food waste. Vegans reported higher on pro-environmental attitudes than vegetarians and omnivores. Vegans/vegetarians differed significantly from omnivores but did not differ significantly from each other when it came to environmental concern. This weak association provides limited evidence that these groups differ when larger scale environmental issues are discussed. Future research would benefit from investigating potential antecedents of household food waste behaviour.

References
  1. Majeed MT, Luni T. Renewable energy, water, and environmental degradation: A global panel data approach. Pakistan J Commerce Social Sci. 2019; 13: 749-778.
  2. Fróna D, Szenderák J, Harangi-Rákos M. The Challenge of Feeding the World. Sustainability. 2019; 11: 5816.
  3. Graham-Smith F. Population, the Complex Reality: A Report of the Population Summit of the World’s Scientific Academies. Fulcrum Publishing. 1994.
  4. Foley JA, Ramankutty N, Brauman KA, Cassidy ES, Gerber JS, Johnston MD, et al. Solutions for a cultivated planet. Nature. 2011; 478: 337-342.
  5. The Economist Intelligence Unit. Tackling food waste: one of the biggest challenges of our time. BCFN Foundation: Food and Nutrition Sustainability Index. 2023.
  6. Quested TE, Marsh E, Stunell D, Parry AD. Spaghetti soup: The complex world of food waste behaviours. Resources, Conservation and Recycling. 2013; 79: 43-51.
  7. Stancu V, Haugaard P, Lähteenmäki L. Determinants of consumer food waste behaviour: Two routes to food waste. Appetite2016; 96: 7-17.
  8. WRAP. Food and drink WRAP UK. 2023.
  9. Shah G, Gode S, Shivarkar S, Wani P. Awareness of Carbon Emission and its               Hazardous Effect Due to Food Waste and Food Supply Chain amongst Residents of           Pune. ATITHYA: A J Hospitality. 2021; 7.
  10. Werf P, Seabrook JA, Gilliland JA. Food for naught: Using the theory of planned behaviour to better understand household food wasting behaviour. The Canadian Geographer / Le Géographe Canadien. 2019; 63: 478-493.
  11. Schanes K, Dobernig K, Gözet B. Food waste matters - A systematic review of household food waste practices and their policy implications. J Cleaner Production. 2018; 182: 978-991.
  12. Ananno AA, Masud MH, Chowdhury SA, Dabnichki P, Ahmed N, Arefin AME. Sustainable food waste management model for Bangladesh. Sustainable Production and Consumption. 2021; 27: 35-51.
  13. The Nest. Food Waste Facts. OzHarvest. 2018.  
  14. Kummu M, de Moel H, Porkka M, Siebert S, Varis O, Ward PJ. Lost food, wasted resources: Global food supply chain losses and their impacts on freshwater, cropland, and fertiliser use. Science of The Total Environment. 2012; 438: 477-489.
  15. Williams H, Wikström F. Environmental impact of packaging and food losses in a life cycle perspective: a comparative analysis of five food items. J Cleaner Production. 2011; 19: 43-48.
  16. Graham-Rowe E, Jessop DC, Sparks P. Predicting household food waste reduction using an extended theory of planned behaviour. Resources, Conservation and Recycling. 2015; 101: 194-202.
  17. Evans D. Blaming the consumer – once again: the social and material contexts of everyday food waste practices in some English households. Critical Public Health. 2011; 21: 429-440.
  18. Watson M, Meah A. Food, Waste and Safety: Negotiating Conflicting Social Anxieties into the Practices of Domestic Provisioning. The Sociological Rev. 2012; 60: 102-120.
  19. Visschers VHM, Wickli N, Siegrist M. Sorting out food waste behaviour: A survey on the motivators and barriers of self-reported amounts of food waste in households. J Environmental Psychol. 2016; 45: 66-78.
  20. Porpino G, Parente J, Wansink B. Food waste paradox: antecedents of food disposal in low income households. Int J Consumer Studies. 2015; 39: 619-629.
  21. Stefan V, van Herpen E, Tudoran AA, Lähteenmäki L. Avoiding food waste by Romanian consumers: The importance of planning and shopping routines. Food Quality and Preference. 2013; 28: 375-381.
  22. Trethewey E, Jackson M. Values and cognitive mechanisms: Comparing the predictive factors of Australian meat intake. Appetite. 2019; 142: 104386.
  23. Department of Primary Industries and Regional Development. Reducing livestock greenhouse gas emissions | Agriculture and Food. 2020.
  24. Veeramani A, Dias GM, Kirkpatrick SI. Carbon footprint of dietary patterns in Ontario, Canada: A case study based on actual food consumption. J Cleaner Production. 2017; 162: 1398-1406.
  25. Beardsworth AD, Keil ET. Vegetarianism, Veganism, and Meat Avoidance: Recent Trends and Findings. British Food J. 1991; 93: 19-24.
  26. Ruby MB. Vegetarianism. A blossoming field of study. Appetite. 2012; 58: 141-150.
  27. Peters R. Veganism Statistics Australia. How many vegans are there? Truly Experiences Blog. 2022.  
  28. Ajzen I. The theory of planned behaviour. Organizational Behaviour and Human Decision Processes. 1991; 50: 179-211.
  29. Russell SV, Young CW, Unsworth KL, Robinson C. Bringing habits and emotions into food waste behaviour. Resources, Conservation and Recycling. 2017; 125: 107-114.
  30. Allen MW, Wilson M, Ng SH, Dunne M. Values and Beliefs of Vegetarians and Omnivores. The J Social Psychol. 2000; 140: 405-422.
  31. Janssen M, Busch C, Rödiger M, Hamm U. Motives of consumers following a vegan diet and their attitudes towards animal agriculture. Appetite. 2016; 105: 643-651.
  32. Hoek AC, Luning PA, Stafleu A, de Graaf C. Food-related lifestyle and health attitudes of Dutch vegetarians, non-vegetarian consumers of meat substitutes, and meat consumers. Appetite. 2004; 42: 265-272.
  33. Lund TB, McKeegan DEF, Cribbin C, Sandøe P. Animal Ethics Profiling of Vegetarians, Vegans and Meat-Eaters. Anthrozoös. 2016; 29: 89-106.
  34. Gatersleben B, Murtagh N, Abrahamse W. Values, identity and pro environmental behaviour. Contemporary Social Sci. 2012; 9: 374-392.
  35. Fisher A, Clayton S, Opotow S. Identity and the Natural Environment: The Psychological Significance of Nature. J Phenomenological Psychol. 2003; 37: 143-148.
  36. Joyce A, Dixon S, Comfort J, Hallett J. Reducing the Environmental Impact of Dietary Choice: Perspectives from a Behavioural and Social Change Approach. J Environ Public Health. 2012; 2012: 1-7.
  37. Williams H, Wikström, F, Otterbring T, Löfgren M, Gustafsson A. Reasons for household food waste with special attention to packaging. J Cleaner Production. 2012; 24: 141-148.
  38. Bryant CJ. We Can’t Keep Meating Like This: Attitudes towards Vegetarian and Vegan Diets in the United Kingdom. Sustainability. 2019; 11: 6844.
  39. Neff RA, Spiker ML, Truant PL. Wasted Food: U.S. Consumers’ Reported Awareness, Attitudes, and Behaviours. PLOS ONE. 2015; 10: e0127881.
  40. Principato L, Secondi L, Pratesi CA. Reducing food waste: an investigation on the behaviour of Italian youths. British Food J. 2015; 117: 731-748.
  41. Qi D, Roe BE. Household Food Waste: Multivariate Regression and Principal Components Analyses of Awareness and Attitudes among U.S. Consumers. PLOS ONE. 2016; 11: e0159250.
  42. Melbye EL, Onozaka Y, Hansen H. Throwing It All Away: Exploring Affluent Consumers’ Attitudes Toward Wasting Edible Food. J Food Products Marketing. 2016; 23: 416-429.
  43. Félonneau ML, Becker M. Pro-environmental attitudes and behaviour: Revealing perceived social desirability. Revue Internationale de Psychologie Sociale, Numéro. 2008; 80: 25-53.
  44. Bissing-Olson MJ, Iyer A, Fielding KS, Zacher H. Relationships between daily affect and pro-environmental behaviour at work: The moderating role of proenvironmental attitude. J Organizational Behav. 2012; 34: 156-175.
  45. Bamberg S, Möser G. Twenty years after Hines, Hungerford, and Tomera: A new meta-analysis of psycho-social determinants of pro-environmental behaviour. J Environ Psychol. 2007; 27: 14-25.
  46. Kaiser FG, Wölfing S, Fuhrer U. ENVIRONMENTAL ATTITUDE AND ECOLOGICAL BEHAVIOUR. J Environ Psychol.1999; 19: 1-19.
  47. Andersson L, Shivarajan S, Blau G. Enacting Ecological Sustainability in the MNC: A Test of an Adapted Value-Belief-Norm Framework. J Business Ethics. 2005; 59: 295-305.
  48. Hinds J, Sparks P. Engaging with the natural environment: The role of affective connection and identity. J Environ Psychol. 2008; 28: 109-120.
  49. Kollmuss A, Agyeman J. Mind the Gap: Why do people act environmentally and what are the barriers to pro-environmental behaviour? Environ Education Res. 2002; 8: 239-260.
  50. Qualtrics (XM). Qualtrics. 2025.
  51. Webb TL, Sheeran P. Does changing behavioral intentions engender behavior change? A meta-analysis of the experimental evidence. Psychol Bull. 2006; 132: 249-268.      
  52. Tilikidou I, Adamson I, Sarmaniotis C. The Measurement Instrument of Ecologically Conscious Consumer Behaviour. MEDIT.2002; 1: 46-53.
  53. Brook Lyndhurst. Food behaviour consumer research findings from the quantitative survey. 2007.
  54. Exodus. We don't waste food. A household survey. Final Report. UK: WRAP. Project code: EVAPP9. 2007.
  55. Chen X, Peterson MN, Hull V, Lu C, Lee GD, Hong D, et al. Effects of attitudinal and sociodemographic factors on pro-environmental behaviour in urban China. Environmental Conservation. 2011; 38: 45-52.
  56. Fielding KS, Head BW. Determinants of young Australians’ environmental actions: the role of responsibility attributions, locus of control, knowledge and attitudes. Environ Education Res. 2012; 18: 171-186.
  57. Maiteny, PT. Mind in the Gap: Summary of research exploring “inner” influences on pro-sustainability learning and behaviour. Environ Education Res 2002; 8: 299-306.
  58. Stern P, Dietz T, Abel T, Guagnano G, Kalof L. A Value-Belief-Norm Theory of Support for Social Movements: The Case of Environmentalism. Human Ecology Rev. 1999; 6: 81-97.
  59. Dunlap RE, Van Liere KD, Mertig AG, Jones RE. New Trends in Measuring Environmental Attitudes: Measuring Endorsement of the New Ecological Paradigm: A Revised NEP Scale. J Social Issues. 2000; 56: 425-442.
  60. Worsley A, Wang WC, Burton, M. Food concerns and support for environmental food policies and purchasing. Appetite. 2015; 91, 48-55.
  61. Campbell-Arvai V. Food-related environmental beliefs and behaviours among university undergraduates. Int J Sustainability in Higher Education. 2015; 16: 279-295.
  62. Lea E, Worsley A. Benefits and barriers to the consumption of a vegetarian diet in Australia. Public Health Nutr. 2003; 6: 505-511.
  63. Vanhonacker F, Van Loo EJ, Gellynck X, Verbeke W. Flemish consumer attitudes towards more sustainable food choices. Appetite. 2013; 62: 7-16.
  64. Povey R, Wellens B, Conner M. Attitudes towards following meat, vegetarian and vegan diets: an examination of the role of ambivalence. Appetite. 2001; 37: 15-26.
  65. Bryant C. Plant-based animal product alternatives are healthier and more environmentally sustainable than animal products. Future Foods. 2022; 6: 100174.
  66. Murray-Ragg N. Meat-Free Diets Make You Less Likely to Get Food Poisoning, Says Study. LIVEKINDLY. 2020.
  67. Jansen TR. Can smartphone apps help solve the world’s food waste problem? Ensia. 2019.
  68. Ajzen, I. Theory of Planned Behaviour Diagram. Theory of Planned Behaviour Diagram. 2019.
  69. Tobler C, Visschers V. HM, Siegrist M. Eating green. Consumers’ willingness to adopt ecological food consumption behaviours. Appetite. 2011; 57: 674-682.
Received : 10 Sep 2025
Accepted : 08 Sep 2025
Published : 10 Sep 2025
Journals
Annals of Otolaryngology and Rhinology
ISSN : 2379-948X
Launched : 2014
JSM Schizophrenia
Launched : 2016
Journal of Nausea
Launched : 2020
JSM Internal Medicine
Launched : 2016
JSM Hepatitis
Launched : 2016
JSM Oro Facial Surgeries
ISSN : 2578-3211
Launched : 2016
JSM Regenerative Medicine and Bioengineering
ISSN : 2379-0490
Launched : 2013
JSM Spine
ISSN : 2578-3181
Launched : 2016
Archives of Palliative Care
ISSN : 2573-1165
Launched : 2016
JSM Nutritional Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3203
Launched : 2017
Annals of Neurodegenerative Disorders
ISSN : 2476-2032
Launched : 2016
Journal of Fever
ISSN : 2641-7782
Launched : 2017
JSM Bone Marrow Research
ISSN : 2578-3351
Launched : 2016
JSM Mathematics and Statistics
ISSN : 2578-3173
Launched : 2014
Journal of Autoimmunity and Research
ISSN : 2573-1173
Launched : 2014
JSM Arthritis
ISSN : 2475-9155
Launched : 2016
JSM Head and Neck Cancer-Cases and Reviews
ISSN : 2573-1610
Launched : 2016
JSM General Surgery Cases and Images
ISSN : 2573-1564
Launched : 2016
JSM Anatomy and Physiology
ISSN : 2573-1262
Launched : 2016
JSM Dental Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1548
Launched : 2016
Annals of Emergency Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1017
Launched : 2016
Annals of Mens Health and Wellness
ISSN : 2641-7707
Launched : 2017
Journal of Preventive Medicine and Health Care
ISSN : 2576-0084
Launched : 2018
Journal of Chronic Diseases and Management
ISSN : 2573-1300
Launched : 2016
Annals of Vaccines and Immunization
ISSN : 2378-9379
Launched : 2014
JSM Heart Surgery Cases and Images
ISSN : 2578-3157
Launched : 2016
Annals of Reproductive Medicine and Treatment
ISSN : 2573-1092
Launched : 2016
JSM Brain Science
ISSN : 2573-1289
Launched : 2016
JSM Biomarkers
ISSN : 2578-3815
Launched : 2014
JSM Biology
ISSN : 2475-9392
Launched : 2016
Archives of Stem Cell and Research
ISSN : 2578-3580
Launched : 2014
Annals of Clinical and Medical Microbiology
ISSN : 2578-3629
Launched : 2014
JSM Pediatric Surgery
ISSN : 2578-3149
Launched : 2017
Journal of Memory Disorder and Rehabilitation
ISSN : 2578-319X
Launched : 2016
JSM Tropical Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2578-3165
Launched : 2016
JSM Head and Face Medicine
ISSN : 2578-3793
Launched : 2016
JSM Cardiothoracic Surgery
ISSN : 2573-1297
Launched : 2016
JSM Bone and Joint Diseases
ISSN : 2578-3351
Launched : 2017
JSM Bioavailability and Bioequivalence
ISSN : 2641-7812
Launched : 2017
JSM Atherosclerosis
ISSN : 2573-1270
Launched : 2016
Journal of Genitourinary Disorders
ISSN : 2641-7790
Launched : 2017
Journal of Fractures and Sprains
ISSN : 2578-3831
Launched : 2016
Journal of Autism and Epilepsy
ISSN : 2641-7774
Launched : 2016
Annals of Marine Biology and Research
ISSN : 2573-105X
Launched : 2014
JSM Health Education & Primary Health Care
ISSN : 2578-3777
Launched : 2016
JSM Communication Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3807
Launched : 2016
Annals of Musculoskeletal Disorders
ISSN : 2578-3599
Launched : 2016
Annals of Virology and Research
ISSN : 2573-1122
Launched : 2014
JSM Renal Medicine
ISSN : 2573-1637
Launched : 2016
Journal of Muscle Health
ISSN : 2578-3823
Launched : 2016
JSM Genetics and Genomics
ISSN : 2334-1823
Launched : 2013
JSM Anxiety and Depression
ISSN : 2475-9139
Launched : 2016
Clinical Journal of Heart Diseases
ISSN : 2641-7766
Launched : 2016
Annals of Medicinal Chemistry and Research
ISSN : 2378-9336
Launched : 2014
JSM Pain and Management
ISSN : 2578-3378
Launched : 2016
JSM Women's Health
ISSN : 2578-3696
Launched : 2016
Clinical Research in HIV or AIDS
ISSN : 2374-0094
Launched : 2013
Journal of Endocrinology, Diabetes and Obesity
ISSN : 2333-6692
Launched : 2013
Journal of Substance Abuse and Alcoholism
ISSN : 2373-9363
Launched : 2013
JSM Neurosurgery and Spine
ISSN : 2373-9479
Launched : 2013
Journal of Liver and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2379-0830
Launched : 2014
Journal of Drug Design and Research
ISSN : 2379-089X
Launched : 2014
JSM Clinical Oncology and Research
ISSN : 2373-938X
Launched : 2013
JSM Bioinformatics, Genomics and Proteomics
ISSN : 2576-1102
Launched : 2014
JSM Chemistry
ISSN : 2334-1831
Launched : 2013
Journal of Trauma and Care
ISSN : 2573-1246
Launched : 2014
JSM Surgical Oncology and Research
ISSN : 2578-3688
Launched : 2016
Annals of Food Processing and Preservation
ISSN : 2573-1033
Launched : 2016
Journal of Radiology and Radiation Therapy
ISSN : 2333-7095
Launched : 2013
JSM Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation
ISSN : 2578-3572
Launched : 2016
Annals of Clinical Pathology
ISSN : 2373-9282
Launched : 2013
Annals of Cardiovascular Diseases
ISSN : 2641-7731
Launched : 2016
Journal of Behavior
ISSN : 2576-0076
Launched : 2016
Annals of Clinical and Experimental Metabolism
ISSN : 2572-2492
Launched : 2016
Clinical Research in Infectious Diseases
ISSN : 2379-0636
Launched : 2013
JSM Microbiology
ISSN : 2333-6455
Launched : 2013
Journal of Urology and Research
ISSN : 2379-951X
Launched : 2014
Journal of Family Medicine and Community Health
ISSN : 2379-0547
Launched : 2013
Annals of Pregnancy and Care
ISSN : 2578-336X
Launched : 2017
JSM Cell and Developmental Biology
ISSN : 2379-061X
Launched : 2013
Annals of Aquaculture and Research
ISSN : 2379-0881
Launched : 2014
Clinical Research in Pulmonology
ISSN : 2333-6625
Launched : 2013
Journal of Immunology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2333-6714
Launched : 2013
Annals of Forensic Research and Analysis
ISSN : 2378-9476
Launched : 2014
JSM Biochemistry and Molecular Biology
ISSN : 2333-7109
Launched : 2013
Annals of Breast Cancer Research
ISSN : 2641-7685
Launched : 2016
Annals of Gerontology and Geriatric Research
ISSN : 2378-9409
Launched : 2014
Journal of Sleep Medicine and Disorders
ISSN : 2379-0822
Launched : 2014
JSM Burns and Trauma
ISSN : 2475-9406
Launched : 2016
Chemical Engineering and Process Techniques
ISSN : 2333-6633
Launched : 2013
Annals of Clinical Cytology and Pathology
ISSN : 2475-9430
Launched : 2014
JSM Allergy and Asthma
ISSN : 2573-1254
Launched : 2016
Journal of Neurological Disorders and Stroke
ISSN : 2334-2307
Launched : 2013
Annals of Sports Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2379-0571
Launched : 2014
JSM Sexual Medicine
ISSN : 2578-3718
Launched : 2016
Annals of Vascular Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2378-9344
Launched : 2014
JSM Biotechnology and Biomedical Engineering
ISSN : 2333-7117
Launched : 2013
Journal of Hematology and Transfusion
ISSN : 2333-6684
Launched : 2013
JSM Environmental Science and Ecology
ISSN : 2333-7141
Launched : 2013
Journal of Cardiology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2333-6676
Launched : 2013
JSM Nanotechnology and Nanomedicine
ISSN : 2334-1815
Launched : 2013
Journal of Ear, Nose and Throat Disorders
ISSN : 2475-9473
Launched : 2016
JSM Ophthalmology
ISSN : 2333-6447
Launched : 2013
Journal of Pharmacology and Clinical Toxicology
ISSN : 2333-7079
Launched : 2013
Annals of Psychiatry and Mental Health
ISSN : 2374-0124
Launched : 2013
Medical Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology
ISSN : 2333-6439
Launched : 2013
Annals of Pediatrics and Child Health
ISSN : 2373-9312
Launched : 2013
JSM Clinical Pharmaceutics
ISSN : 2379-9498
Launched : 2014
JSM Foot and Ankle
ISSN : 2475-9112
Launched : 2016
JSM Alzheimer's Disease and Related Dementia
ISSN : 2378-9565
Launched : 2014
Journal of Addiction Medicine and Therapy
ISSN : 2333-665X
Launched : 2013
Journal of Veterinary Medicine and Research
ISSN : 2378-931X
Launched : 2013
Annals of Public Health and Research
ISSN : 2378-9328
Launched : 2014
Annals of Orthopedics and Rheumatology
ISSN : 2373-9290
Launched : 2013
Journal of Clinical Nephrology and Research
ISSN : 2379-0652
Launched : 2014
Annals of Community Medicine and Practice
ISSN : 2475-9465
Launched : 2014
Annals of Biometrics and Biostatistics
ISSN : 2374-0116
Launched : 2013
JSM Clinical Case Reports
ISSN : 2373-9819
Launched : 2013
Journal of Cancer Biology and Research
ISSN : 2373-9436
Launched : 2013
Journal of Surgery and Transplantation Science
ISSN : 2379-0911
Launched : 2013
Journal of Dermatology and Clinical Research
ISSN : 2373-9371
Launched : 2013
JSM Gastroenterology and Hepatology
ISSN : 2373-9487
Launched : 2013
Annals of Nursing and Practice
ISSN : 2379-9501
Launched : 2014
JSM Dentistry
ISSN : 2333-7133
Launched : 2013
Author Information X